Abstract
When holding visual information temporarily in working memory (WM), the neural representation of the memorandum is distributed across various cortical regions, including visual and frontal cortices. However, the role of stimulus representation in visual and frontal cortices during WM has been controversial. Here we tested the hypothesis that stimulus representation persists in the frontal cortex to facilitate flexible control demands in WM. During functional MRI, participants flexibly switched between simple WM maintenance of visual stimulus or more complex rule-based categorization of maintained stimulus on a trial-by-trial basis. Our results demonstrated enhanced stimulus representation in the frontal cortex that tracked demands for active WM control and enhanced stimulus representation in the visual cortex that tracked demands for precise WM maintenance. This differential frontal stimulus representation traded off with the newly-generated category representation with varying control demands. Simulation using multi-module recurrent neural networks replicated human neural patterns when stimulus information was preserved for network readout. Altogether, these findings help reconcile the long-standing debate in WM research, and provide empirical and computational evidence that flexible stimulus representation in the frontal cortex during WM serves as a potential neural coding scheme to accommodate the ever-changing environment.
Introduction
Real-world flexible behavior relies largely on working memory (WM), which allows the maintenance and manipulation of information in the brain in order to serve diverse behavioral goals (Baddeley, 2003). One central problem in the field of WM is to understand how stimulus information is represented and maintained in WM. Over the past decade, mounting evidence has demonstrated stimulus-specific representation during WM maintenance in a distributed cortical network, including sensory, parietal, and frontal cortices (Christophel et al., 2012; Ester et al., 2015; Gosseries et al., 2018; Harrison & Tong, 2009; Riggall & Postle, 2012; Serences et al., 2009; Sprague & Serences, 2013; Yu & Shim, 2017, 2019). However, the exact nature and functions of stimulus representation in different cortical regions remain controversial. Specifically, while neurophysiological studies in non-human primates have mostly emphasized stimulus representation in the frontal cortex (Funahashi et al., 1989; Fuster & Alexander, 1971; Leavitt et al., 2017), neuroimaging work in humans has reported disparate findings. During maintenance of simple visual features, stimulus representation is robustly encoded in the early visual cortex (EVC), which has been taken as the evidence in support of the sensorimotor recruitment hypothesis of WM (Harrison & Tong, 2009; Riggall & Postle, 2012; Serences et al., 2009). Meanwhile, those in the higher-order frontoparietal cortex are typically weaker and less stable (Emrich et al., 2013; Gosseries et al., 2018; Riggall & Postle, 2012; Yu & Shim, 2019). However, in dynamic environments such as those involving distraction, stimulus representation in EVC could be greatly interrupted or biased (Bettencourt & Xu, 2016; Hallenbeck et al., 2021; Lorenc et al., 2018). In contrast, stimulus representation in the frontal cortex could be robust under certain circumstances including attentional prioritization (Christophel et al., 2018), categorization (Lee et al., 2013) and after extensive training (Miller et al., 2022). To summarize, stimulus representation could vary markedly depending on specific brain regions and memory tasks, complicating the interpretation of potential functions of stimulus representation in WM.
In this study, we consider these apparent discrepancies from the perspective of cognitive flexibility (Badre et al., 2021; Fusi et al., 2016; Musslick & Cohen, 2021). We propose that changes in stimulus representation in different cortical regions might reflect a global reconfiguration in coding strategy and resource allocation in response to varied WM functions (Henderson et al., 2022; Lee et al., 2013). To elaborate, beyond the passive maintenance of incoming sensory information, WM provides an online mental workspace for active manipulation and control of stimulus contents (Baddeley, 2003; Miller & Cohen, 2001). As control functions often result in the generation and maintenance of new information, the brain needs to manage not only the original stimulus information but also the newly generated information in WM. Due to the limited cognitive resources available, it is likely that original stimulus representation in WM could adapt flexibly to various task goals beyond simple maintenance of WM contents, which might also co-vary with changes in the representation of the newly-generated information, leading to a systematic reconfiguration in representations of all levels across various cortices. We make two specific predictions from this account. First, in accordance with the findings of elevated neural activity in the frontal cortex with increasing demand for memory manipulation (D’Esposito et al., 1999; D’Esposito et al., 2000) and cognitive control (Badre, 2008; Badre et al., 2010; Miller & Cohen, 2001), stimulus representation in frontal cortex should be enhanced for active-control-related functions in WM. By contrast, due to the precise nature of stimulus representation in visual cortex, stimulus representation in this region should be enhanced for precise-maintenance-related functions in WM (Henderson et al., 2022; Lee et al., 2013). Second, within the brain regions that encode the newly generated information, a dynamic tradeoff between representations of original and new information should be observed to achieve flexible allocation of limited cognitive resources (Badre et al., 2021; Flesch et al., 2022).
Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we directly tested this account by systematically investigating stimulus representation in visual, parietal, and frontal cortices during WM tasks with varying demands for active control. In particular, we surmised that stimulus representation in the frontal cortex would increase to accommodate complex control demands such as rule-based categorization. To this end, we employed a visual WM paradigm that required flexible switching between maintenance and categorization tasks. Specifically, in the maintenance task, participants maintained one visual orientation throughout a delay period, whereas in the categorization task participants were required to categorize the remembered orientation into one of two categories in accordance with previously learned rules, which could be either switched randomly between two rules on a block-by-block basis (Experiment 1) or fixed with one rule (Experiment 2). Thus, compared with the maintenance task, the categorization task imposed additional control demand of WM information at two different levels.
In line with our prediction, our results showed that stimulus information was more prominently represented in the frontal cortex during categorization than maintenance task, and this differential representation was enhanced with increasing demands for control. Importantly, the strength of stimulus representation in the frontal cortex was predictive of WM behavioral performance in the categorization but not in the maintenance task, implicating a selective involvement of the frontal cortex in control functions. By contrast, stimulus representation in the visual cortex was found to exhibit an opposite pattern, with higher strength for maintenance than categorization task. Moreover, varying control demands across experiments revealed a dynamic tradeoff between stimulus and the newly-generated category representations. To further examine whether the enhanced stimulus representation in the frontal cortex during categorization task could be explained by global coding strategy, we simulated our flexible WM tasks with multi-module recurrent neural networks (RNNs). The results of this computational modeling well replicated our human data when precise stimulus information was preserved at the output during network training. Taken together, our results indicate the importance of the frontal cortex for flexible control in WM and highlight the relative changes of stimulus representation in different cortical regions for varying task demands of WM.
Results
Behavioral learning and performance of WM tasks
In the fMRI session of Experiment 1, human participants (n = 24) completed two tasks, maintenance and categorization, inside an MRI scanner. The maintenance task was a delayed match-to-sample working memory task of orientations. Participants only needed to maintained the cued orientation throughout a memory delay. In categorization task, participants also started with maintaining an orientation. After the task cue, they needed to categorize the remembered orientation into one of two categories using the cued categorization rule. Within an experimental block of nine trials, participants randomly switched between the two tasks. Across blocks, participants randomly switched between two categorization rules acquired during a preceding learning session. We randomized response mapping across trials to avoid potential influence by motor-planning signals (see Figure 1A for probe design). Prior to the main session, participants first completed a behavioral learning session to learn two categorization rules (Rule A and Rule B, see Figure 1B) that were orthogonal to each other. Participants acquired the two rules with equal familiarity (t(23) = 0.24, p = 0.813; for Rule A, M = 0.85, SD = 0.050; for Rule B, M = 0.85, SD = 0.05; Figure 1C) and comparable precision (averaged error in reported boundaries for Rule A were 8.80° ± 6.29° and that for Rule B was 9.02° ± 5.69°; Figure 1D).
Overall, participants performed equally well on both tasks in the fMRI session. Accuracy for the maintenance task (M ± SD: 0.81 ± 0.07) and that for the categorization task (0.82 ± 0.05) did not significantly differ (t(23) = 1.51, p = 0.144; Figure 1E), suggesting that the two tasks were matched in terms of task difficulty. In line with previous categorization studies (Ester et al., 2020; Freedman & Assad, 2006) demonstrating a boundary effect, only in the categorization task but not in the maintenance task did participants perform better for trials distant from category boundaries in terms of both accuracy and reaction time (see Figure 1G). These results demonstrated the effect of categorization and confirmed that participants faithfully followed task instructions.
Enhanced stimulus representation in frontal cortex during categorization task
The primary goal of this study was to determine the role of stimulus representation in various cortices in WM. Using conventional multivariate encoding and decoding methods, we tracked stimulus (i.e., orientation) representation in three brain regions of interest (ROIs) that have been implicated in representing WM information, including early visual cortex (EVC), intraparietal sulcus (IPS), and superior precentral sulcus (sPCS) (Christophel et al., 2018; Ester et al., 2015; Hallenbeck et al., 2021; Yu & Shim, 2017).
First, we used multivariate inverted encoding models (IEMs) (Brouwer & Heeger, 2009, 2011; Ester et al., 2015; Rademaker et al., 2019; Yu & Shim, 2017) to reconstruct orientation representation at the population level in each ROI. Figure 2A shows example orientation reconstructions from representative time points, and Figure 2C demonstrates the time course of orientation reconstruction as quantified by representational fidelity. A larger fidelity value indicates a stronger orientation representation (Figure 2B). In EVC, we found significant orientation representation in both maintenance and categorization tasks starting from the sample period (Figure 2C left panel; see Supplemental Table 1 for full statistics), even when the categorization task did not require explicit memory of stimulus information. Additionally, the strength of orientation representation in the maintenance task became significantly higher than that in the categorization task after the task cue during the delay, suggesting the strength of orientation representations in EVC reflected the degrees of task demands for maintaining visual details. In IPS, orientation representation was significant in both tasks, but did not differ from each other at most time points (Figure 2C middle panel). In sPCS, a reversed pattern was observed. In the maintenance task, orientation information was maintained during early delay period and then dropped to baseline level during late delay period. By contrast, in the categorization task, orientation representation was persistent throughout the delay and response periods. The strength of orientation representation in the categorization task became statistically higher than that in the maintenance task in late delay period (Figure 2C right panel), suggesting that this differential representations of visual stimulus in the frontal cortex reflected the demand for active control of memory contents. To facilitate comparison of the differential stimulus representation across ROIs, we averaged the difference in representational strength across a late task epoch (11 – 16 s), and the difference in stimulus representation between ROIs remained (Figure 2D).
We validated the difference in stimulus representations through a series of control analyses. First, we demonstrated that these results cannot be explained by the specific model used to train the data (Liu et al., 2018; Sprague et al., 2018) nor the specific analytical approach used, because similar patterns were observed when we trained the IEM separately for each condition (Figure S2A) or adopted a Support Vector Machine (SVM) decoding approach (Figure S2D) (Henderson et al., 2022; Rademaker et al., 2019). Mean activation differences between tasks cannot account for the results either, because when we removed the mean differences in BOLD activity between tasks, the difference in representational strength remained (Figure S2C). Furthermore, to remove the potential impact of voxel number on IEM, we selected the top 500 of most sample- or delay-selective voxels from each ROI and trained IEM using the selected voxels. Again, this analysis yielded similar findings (Figure S2B). Together, these results demonstrated enhanced stimulus representation in the frontal cortex with increased demand for active control, as well as those in the visual cortex with increased demand for precise WM maintenance.
Prediction of categorization behavior by frontal stimulus representation
Previous WM studies have shown that the strength of stimulus representation in EVC positively correlated with memory performance (Emrich et al., 2013; Ester et al., 2013; Gosseries et al., 2018), suggesting that EVC plays an important role in precise WM maintenance. However, stimulus representation in the frontal cortex rarely predicted behavioral performance in maintenance task (Hallenbeck et al., 2021). Nevertheless, if frontal stimulus representation is involved in WM control, its behavioral relevance should be subject to observation with increased control demands. Therefore, we assessed the behavioral predictability of stimulus representation during the delay period in EVC, IPS, and sPCS (Figure 3). Consistent with previous findings, we found the strength of stimulus representation in EVC during the early delay period predicted behavioral accuracies in both maintenance and categorization tasks (see Supplemental Table 2 for full statistics). Similar predictability was found in IPS, with stimulus representation predicted behavior in the maintenance task during early delay and in the categorization task throughout the entire memory delay. Interestingly, we found that, throughout the entire memory delay, the strength of stimulus representation in sPCS predicted behavioral accuracies only in the categorization task but not in the maintenance task. These results highlighted the functional significance of stimulus representation in sPCS exclusively for the categorization task.
Reduced frontal stimulus representation with lower control demand
In Experiment 1, participants flexibly switched between two categorization rules to prompt the manipulation of WM content on a trial-by-trial basis. The rule switching increased control demand but also complicated the interpretation of our results. To exclude potential impact of rule switching, we conducted Experiment 2, in which participants performed maintenance and categorization tasks with only one fixed rule. Behavioral results of Experiment 2 again demonstrated a classic boundary effect and were comparable to Experiment 1 (Figure S1), with no significant difference between experiments in terms of either accuracy or reaction time (Fs < 1.28, ps > 0.26). When using IEMs to reconstruct stimulus representation, we found EVC and IPS both showed patterns similar to those in Experiment 1 (Figure 4A), with stimulus representation decreased in EVC in categorization task and remained at the same level in IPS between the two tasks (see Supplemental Table 3 for full statistics). The frontal region, sPCS, also showed a differential enhancement of stimulus representation in categorization task as in Experiment 1, but in an earlier delay period (Figure 4A). To validate such a temporal difference, we defined an additional early task period (5 – 10 s), and confirmed a significant difference in stimulus representation in sPCS during the early (p = 0.015; Figure 4B) but not during the late epoch (p = 0.372; Figure 4C). In addition, we performed a mixed ANOVA on experiments (Experiment 1 vs. 2) and epochs (early vs. late epoch) and observed a significant interaction effect between the two, F(1, 46) = 7.43, p = 0.009, suggesting that the two experiments differed in terms of the temporal emergence of the differential stimulus representation in the frontal cortex. Taken together, these results are consistent with our expectation that, with reduced control demand, the differential enhancement of stimulus representation in frontal cortex was still present but decreased during late memory delay. Nevertheless, stimulus representation in Experiment 2 still predicted behavioral performance as in Experiment 1, although the difference between task was reduced (Figure S3).
Category representation in WM in various cortices
Having observed a differential representation of stimulus in the frontal cortex, we next asked how newly generated information in WM during the categorization task emerged and sustained in the distributed WM network and how representations of the original stimulus and new information interacted. The categorization task could demand additional generation of category information in WM. We therefore trained SVMs to decode category information during the categorization task. For each rule, the SVM decoder was trained to discriminate between the two categories. In both experiments, we found that during the late epoch, category information could be well decoded across ROIs (ps < 0.044, Figure 5A; also see Figure S4A for full decoding time course), with a marginal difference between experiments in sPCS (p = 0.055).
One might argue that the category decoding results could at least be partially attributed to stimulus similarity. To minimize the impact of stimulus similarity on category decoding, we additionally trained another decoder using the opposite rule (i.e., using category labels from the orthogonal rule). We then calculated an abstract category index by subtracting decoding accuracy under the opposite rule from that under the true rule (Mok & Love, 2020) (Figure 5B). The rationale was that the amount of stimulus similarity would be comparable for the opposite rule, but additional category information, if existed, should result in higher decoding accuracy for the true rule. After removing stimulus-related signals, average decoding performance of abstract category was only evident in Experiment 2 (ps < 0.017) but not in Experiment 1 (ps > 0.14) for all ROIs. Moreover, decoding performance of abstract category was significantly higher in Experiment 2 than Experiment 1 in sPCS (p = 0.034; Figure 5C). These results together suggest a potential tradeoff between stimulus difference and category representation in the frontal cortex.
Differential stimulus representation in frontal cortex replicated by RNN modeling
Lastly, we tested how stimulus representation could emerge in frontal cortex at the mechanistic level using recurrent neural network (RNN) models. Our hypothesis is that precise stimulus representation during WM might emerge in frontal cortex in response to complex task demands such as rule-based categorization. In other words, instead of relying (solely) on category representations, the cortical network might have adopted a different strategy to accommodate the flexible task requirements in the current study, for instance, by preserving stimulus information until a later stage of information processing. This different strategy can be implemented by altering the RNN’s output structure. Therefore, the logic of this modeling analysis was to examine whether explicitly placing a demand for the model to preserve stimulus representation would recapitulate our fMRI findings in frontal cortex, in comparison to a model that did not specify such a demand.
Two types of modular RNNs were trained on the maintenance and categorization tasks simultaneously (Masse et al., 2019; Zhou et al., 2021). The networks shared common input and hidden layer structures (i.e., orientation-tuned and retro/task cue-related units as the input layer, recurrent units with short-term synaptic plasticity in the hidden layer (80% excitatory + 20% inhibitory units, equally distributed in three separate modules). The only difference was in the structure of the output layer. The first type of RNN (RNN1; n = 20) had only two units in the output layer to indicate networks’ choice (Figure 6A), whereas the second type of RNN (RNN2; n = 20) had additional units in the output layer corresponding to the original stimulus information. In other words, the second RNN was designed to maintain stimulus information throughout the network modules. For the common hidden layer, we included three hierarchically organized (posterior, middle, and anterior) modules of recurrent units generated according to neurobiological principles of neuronal connections (e.g., denser connectivity within than between modules) to simulate the interconnected brain areas in our ROI-based fMRI analyses above: the posterior module (Module 1, simulating EVC) was directly connected with the input layer, the middle module (Module 2, simulating IPS) received projections from the posterior module and relaying information to the anterior module, and the anterior module (Module 3, simulating sPCS) projected to the output layer. Task events were simulated as numerical inputs to the model, matching the procedures of Experiment 1 (see Methods for details).
After successful training, defined as reaching at least 90% accuracies in all tasks in the same training batch, we applied an SVM decoding approach to investigate population-level stimulus representations in neuronal spiking activities of the RNNs. We found that in RNN1, both the middle and anterior modules showed stronger stimulus representation in the maintenance task than the categorization task during the delay period (pposterior = 0.09, pmiddle = 0.011, panterior = 0.007; Figure 6B and Figure S7A), opposite to our fMRI observation in IPS and sPCS. In comparison, decoding performance in RNN2, which was explicitly required to maintain stimulus information for the output, yielded results consistent with our human findings, with increased stimulus decoding performance during categorization only in the anterior module (pposterior = 0.436, pmiddle = 0.212, panterior = 0.026; Figure 6B).
Besides difference in stimulus representation, we further tested whether RNN2 could also replicate the human results on category representation. For this analysis we focused on abstract category representation to fully remove the impact of stimulus on category decoding. To examine the influence of control demand on category decoding, following our fMRI experiment we trained 20 additional RNNs with the same output structure as RNN2 (preserving stimulus information) to perform the tasks with a fixed categorization rule, mimicking the task structure of Experiment 2. Consistent with our human findings, we observed increased abstract category decoding performance in the fixed-rule RNNs compared to the flexible-rule RNNs, throughout the modules (pposterior = 0.045, pmiddle = 0.003, panterior < 0.001; Figure 6C). By contrast, abstract category decoding in RNN1 across modules demonstrated a distinct pattern from human data, with numerically increasing decoding accuracy towards later modules (Figure 6C).
Altogether, these findings demonstrated that our fMRI results could be simulated by RNN models when stimulus information for readout was preserved, suggesting that the requirement for flexible control of WM content could demand high-fidelity stimulus representation at the output stage of the model. Notably, we found that RNN2 generally took less iterations for training and had fewer failures in learning the task (with a defined maximal number of iterations).
Discussion
In this study, we investigated the emergence and maintenance of stimulus representation with varied control demands of WM. In a distributed human cortical network encompassing visual, parietal, and frontal cortex, we found enhanced stimulus representations in the frontal cortex that tracked increasing demands on active WM control, as well as enhanced stimulus representations in the visual cortex that tracked the demand for the precise maintenance of WM content. The enhanced stimulus representation in frontal cortex was well predicted by RNNs that preserved stimulus information for readout at the output stage. Together, these results highlight the unique and critical contributions of stimulus representations in different cortical regions for distinct aspects of WM, and help to resolve the current controversy in the roles of various cortices in WM.
Role of visual cortex in WM maintenance
The visual cortex has been considered a critical site for maintaining visual WM in the context of sensorimotor recruitment hypothesis (D’Esposito & Postle, 2015; Harrison & Tong, 2009). This idea, however, has been challenged in recent years due to some seemingly contradictory findings from the human neuroimaging studies. For example, compared to the frontoparietal cortex, mnemonic representations in EVC were found to be more vulnerable to distractors (Bettencourt & Xu, 2016; Hallenbeck et al., 2021; Lorenc et al., 2018). The decodability of memory contents in visual cortex also depends on the specific task type. A previous study showed that in nonvisual tasks that required judgements on object category instead of visual details, memory contents were no longer decodable in the visual cortex (Lee et al., 2013). In this study, we found that, although the strength of stimulus representation in EVC differed between WM maintenance and categorization tasks, a copy of stimulus representation remained in EVC during the categorization task. Moreover, stimulus representations in both tasks were equally predictive of subsequent memory performance, suggesting the functional significance of EVC representations in WM.
The discrepancy between our results and that of the previous work (Lee et al., 2013) could be attributed to the fact that our categorization task required participants to manipulate remembered information according to arbitrary yet flexible categorization rules, rather than simply paying selective attention to different aspects (visual details vs. category membership) of everyday objects. In our case, maintaining visual details of the memoranda was critical for accurate behavioral responses. Our finding is consistent with the prediction of sensorimotor recruitment hypothesis that representation of memory contents in the visual cortex is necessary for the precise maintenance of visual information. The observation of robust category representation in early visual cortex during the response period further indicated the recruitment of EVC in categorization, possibly for boundary comparison and rule implementation. In fact, our results are consistent with a recent study demonstrating significant stimulus representation in EVC even when memoranda had been transformed into a motor format (Henderson et al., 2022). In addition, electrophysiological research in non-human primates has also shown robust feature selectivity in the visual cortex during a categorization task (Brincat et al., 2018), and recent computational modeling work has suggested intact maintenance of sensory information during categorical judgements (Luu & Stocker, 2021).
Role of frontal cortex in active WM control
Compared to the prominent role of EVC in memory maintenance, sPCS in the frontal cortex played a dominant role in WM tasks that require active control of memory contents such as categorization. Although stimulus representations in sPCS have been observed during WM in previous studies, the nature of these representations remained debatable. In WM tasks that required mere maintenance of memoranda, stimulus was not always decodable in the frontal cortex (Emrich et al., 2013; Gosseries et al., 2018; Riggall & Postle, 2012), raising the issue of functional significance of stimulus representation in the frontal cortex. On the other hand, stimulus representation in the frontal cortex could become robust in the face of tasks that require attentional prioritization and extensive training (Bettencourt & Xu, 2016; Christophel et al., 2018; Hallenbeck et al., 2021; Lorenc et al., 2018; Miller et al., 2022). Our current study contributes to the resolution of this issue by demonstrating that stimulus representation in sPCS increased with increasing demands for WM control. This finding is in line with recent computational studies proposing that active WM functions may involve neuronal mechanisms different from that for passive maintenance. For example, passive maintenance could rely mainly on synaptic plasticity mechanisms, whereas active control functions such as distractor resistance and information manipulation involve more neuronal spiking activity (Masse et al., 2019; Wang, 2021). In this study, we provided the first empirical evidence that the frontal cortex exhibits enhanced stimulus representation in categorization task requiring active WM control and this representation is predictive of WM performance. In contrast, stimulus representation of WM maintenance failed to predict WM performance at high control demand. It would be of interest to further investigate whether this active control in the frontal cortex could be generalized to tasks that require other types of WM control such as mental rotation.
WM representations in frontal cortex support cognitive flexibility
Our results in the frontal cortex are also in line with recent theoretical proposals in the field of cognitive flexibility. To behave flexibly in complex environments with limited cognitive resources, two mechanisms have been proposed: low-dimensional abstraction of stimulus representation for generalization and efficient learning, and high-dimensional stimulus representation for separability and flexible readout (Badre et al., 2021; Flesch et al., 2022; Fusi et al., 2016). Within this framework, high-dimensional stimulus representations during WM might emerge in the frontal cortex in response to complex control demands such as rule-based categorization. The results of the two fMRI experiments in the current study jointly demonstrate a dynamic tradeoff between high-dimensional stimulus and low-dimensional category representations depending on the control demand. Specifically, when control demand was reduced with a single categorization rule in Experiment 2 compared to Experiment 1, the differential stimulus representation in the frontal cortex was also reduced during the late delay period, accompanied by an increase in category decoding performance especially in the frontal cortex. This result is consistent with neurophysiological findings in non-human primates: while robust category selectivity was observed in frontoparietal cortex during the delay period of categorization tasks when the animal was trained on the categorization task only (Brincat et al., 2018; Freedman & Assad, 2006; Freedman et al., 2001; McKee et al., 2014), category selectivity in the parietal cortex was significantly reduced when the animal had been exposed to a maintenance task prior to categorization training (Latimer & Freedman, 2023). Our RNN simulation further confirmed that this dynamic reconfiguration in information coding at the network level can be well explained by a change in the coding strategy for the network readout. In other words, in flexible environments, and with rich prior experience, the brain might adopt an entirely different strategy for processing information in WM. High-dimensional stimulus information might be preserved in its original identity in the higher-order cortex, potentially reducing processing demands in dealing with each task and thereby facilitating efficiency and flexibility (Badre et al., 2021; Flesch et al., 2022; Fusi et al., 2016). One important future direction would be to further address the meta-control mechanisms that determine the flexible selection of coding strategies for WM (Eppinger et al., 2021).
Differentiating between frontal and parietal cortex in WM functions
While many previous WM studies have focused on the functional distinction between sensory and frontoparietal cortex, it has remained less clear how frontal and parietal cortex might differ in terms of WM functions. Some studies have reported stimulus representations with similar functionality in frontal and parietal cortex (Christophel et al., 2018; Yu & Shim, 2019), while others have observed differential patterns (Hu & Yu, 2023; Lee et al., 2013; Li et al., 2023). We interpret the differential patterns as reflecting a difference in the potential origin of the corresponding cognitive functions. For example, in our study, sPCS demonstrated the most prominent effect for enhanced stimulus representation during categorization as well as the tradeoff between stimulus difference and category representation, suggesting that sPCS might serve as the source region for such effects. On the other hand, IPS did show visually similar patterns to sPCS in some analyses. For instance, stimulus representation in IPS was visually but not statistically higher in the categorization task. These results together support the view that our findings in sPCS do not occur in isolation, but rather reflect a dynamic reconfiguration of functional gradients along the cortical hierarchy from early visual to parietal and then to frontal cortex.
Conclusion
In conclusion, we observed a distributed cortical network, including early visual, parietal, and frontal cortex, in representing stimulus-specific information in WM. These stimulus representations in visual and frontal cortex played distinct functional roles, with those in EVC contributing primarily to precise maintenance and those in frontal cortex contributing primarily to active control in WM. RNN simulations indicated that the stimulus representation in the frontal cortex might have emerged as a result of output selection to facilitate cognitive flexibility. Collectively, these results help to reconcile current debates on the functional roles of different cortical regions in WM, and provide new insights into how a unified WM framework could support varied control demands.
Methods
Participants
A total of 54 participants were recruited at Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shanghai Branch. Twenty-six healthy participants (21 female, all right-handed, mean age = 24.0 ± 1.4 years) were recruited for Experiment 1. Two were excluded due to failure in completing the experiment or low conformity to task instructions, remaining 24 participants who completed the main experiment (19 female, mean age = 23.92 ± 1.41 years). Twenty-eight (22 female, mean age = 24.14 ± 1.51 years) participants were recruited for Experiment 2. Two quitted after behavioral training and two did not finish scanning due to technical problems with the scanner, resulting in 24 participants (20 female, mean age = 24.13 ± 1.60 years) in the final analyses. All participants were neurologically healthy and eligible for MRI, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, provided written informed consent approved by the Ethics Committee of Institute of Neuroscience, Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, and were monetarily compensated for their participation. Sample sizes were not estimated a priori but were comparable and even superior to those in previous studies.
Stimuli and Procedure
All stimuli were generated and presented using MATLAB (The MathWorks) and Psychtoolbox 3 extensions (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). During behavioral training, stimuli were presented on a ThinkVision monitor at a viewing distance of 45 cm. Behavioral responses were acquired with a keyboard. During scanning, stimuli were projected onto a SinoRad monitor (1280 x 1024 pixels, refreshing at 60 Hz) viewed through a coil-mounted mirror in the scanner at a viewing distance of 90.5 cm. Participants’ behavioral responses were acquired with a Sinorad MRI-compatible button box.
Behavioral Training
In Experiment 1, prior to scanning, participants were trained to learn two novel rules, Rule A and Rule B, for categorizing orientations. Thirty oriented bars were used as sample stimuli, ranging from 5° to 179° (in increments of 6°; two participants used another set of thirty orientations ranging from 4° to 178°). Each abstract rule was constructed by two orthogonal boundaries that divided the thirty orientations into two categories with fifteen orientations each. Rule A and Rule B were orthogonal to each other. Corresponding boundaries were 20°/110° and 65°/155° (15°/105° and 60°/150° for the two participants using different stimuli sets).
Participants learned new rules through a rule learning task. Each run of learning started with a rule disk informing the target rule. To avoid any potential verbal coding, rule specifics were visually illustrated as rule disks containing two distinct colors (colors randomly assigned to categories every time; see Figure 1A for an example). Rule disk was presented on the screen for 2 s followed by 1 s of fixation. On each trial, an oriented bar (radius=7°) was presented for 1 s followed by a delay of 1 s. Participants were instructed to report the category of the orientation by pressing a response key (‘F’ or ‘J’). To avoid category-response mapping, we randomized the relationship between categories and key buttons across trials. Moreover, to avoid presenting rule boundaries explicitly, we presented key names at random positions within the range defining each category. In other words, participants had to memorize the exact rule boundaries as accurately as possible in order to find the correct key buttons for each trial. Feedback was given at the end of each trial to assist learning.
Participants completed 30 learning trials in each run. They reviewed rule disks after every 10 trials for memory reconsolidation. Each participant completed at least two runs for each rule. After achieving an average accuracy above 86% (26 out of 30 trials) for the first rule, they proceeded to learn the other rule and then to practice the main task for scanning (see next section). Learning order of rules was counterbalanced across subjects. Upon completion of practicing, participants needed to report the boundaries of learned rules as a qualification of behavioral training. If the total error in reported boundaries had exceeded 20°, participants had one more chance to learn by repeating the learning task. Two participants completed one additional behavioral training to recap rule knowledge prior to scanning.
Behavioral training for Experiment 2 followed the same procedure and used the identical sample set (30 orientations ranging from 4° to 178°) as Experiment 1, except that only one rule was trained. Half of the participants in Experiment 2 learned rule boundaries of 19°/109°; the other half learned rule boundaries of 67°/157°.
Flexible WM Task (fMRI Task)
In Experiment 1, during scanning, participants completed a flexible WM task which implemented levels of control demand with different rules. To be specific, participants randomly switched between a maintenance task and a categorization task. In the maintenance task, participants needed to memorize stimulus information (i.e., orientations). In the categorization task, participants needed to categorize orientations following the rule that was randomly assigned and cued on a block basis. Procedure of the main task was visualized in Figure 1A. At the beginning of each block, participants were presented with a rule disk for 3 s, followed by a 2-s interval, instructing the categorization rule of the current block. For each trial, participants saw two oriented bars presented successively. Each bar was presented for 0.75 s, with an inter-stimulus-interval of 0.5 s. Sample sets were the same as those used in behavioral training. After a 0.5-s interval, a retro-cue occurred for 0.5 s, indicating the orientation of which participants should remember. After a 1.5-s delay, a task cue was displayed at fixation for 0.5 s, following by an 8-s memory delay. The task cue was either a letter ‘P’ on maintenance trials, instructing participants to maintain the cued orientation during memory delay as precisely as possible; or the task cue was a letter ‘C’ on categorization trials, asking participants to categorize the cued orientation using the block rule during the delay. Then, participants were probed to respond within 2 s. On maintenance trials, participants needed to select the memorized orientation from two probe orientations; while on categorization trials, participants needed to report the category of the cued orientation. Response mapping followed the same operation as in the learning tasks. Inter-trial-intervals were randomly selected from 3, 5, and 7 s with an equal trial number, resulting in an average trial length of 20 s. Participants switched to the next block after every six categorization trials and three maintenance trials. Each run contained two blocks (i.e., 18 trials).
In Experiment 2, to isolate potential effect of rule switching, the categorization rule stayed the same throughout the experiment. In Experiment 2, participants randomly switched between the maintenance task and the categorization task. Each trial followed the same procedure as Experiment 1.
In Experiment 1, orientations, tasks, and cued target order (1st or 2nd) were counterbalanced across trials, resulting in an equal trial number of 90 across all three conditions (categorization-Rule A, categorization-Rule B, and maintenance). Nineteen out of the twenty-four participants completed 15 runs of the main task. One participant completed 13 runs due to technical difficulties with the scanner. Another four participants completed 30 runs across two scan sessions. The same counterbalancing procedure was conducted for Experiment 2 (90 trials for maintenance task and 180 trials for categorization task). In Experiment 2, six participants completed 15 runs of 18 trials; the other seven completed 18 runs of 15 trials each due to scanner limitations. At the end of scanning, participants reported the rule boundaries three times as a final check of their rule memory.
Data Acquisition
MRI data of Experiment 1 were collected using a 3 Tesla Siemens MRI scanner (Tim Trio; Siemens Healthineers) with a 32-channel head coil at the Functional Brain Imaging Platform at Institute of Neuroscience, Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences. Functional scanning was performed using a gradient-echo echo-planar sequence with the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 1000 ms; echo time (TE) = 30 ms; flip angle (FA) = 40°; voxel size = 3 x 3 x 3 mm; multi-band accelerate factor = 4; matrix size = 74 x 74; slice number = 60. A high-resolution anatomical T1 image was collected before functional scanning (TR = 2300 ms; TE = 2.98 ms; FOV = 256 x 240 x 192 mm; voxel size = 1 x 1 x 1 mm). During scanning, participants’ head positions were restricted with surrounding paddings to prevent head movements. MRI data of Experiment 2 were collected using identical procedure and settings except that the last eleven participants were scanned using a newly installed 3 Tesla Siemens MRI scanner (Prisma; Siemens Healthineers) at the Functional Brain Imaging Platform.
Preprocessing
All preprocessing of individual MRI data was performed using AFNI (afni.nimh.nih.gov) (Cox, 1996; Cox & Hyde, 1997). Functional data of all runs were registered to the last volume of the final run with the first eight volumes of each run removed. Then, individuals’ aligned functional data were registered to their corresponding T1 volume. Alignment of registration was manually checked for each subject to ensure quality. The registered data were further motion corrected and detrended.
ROI Definition
Our primary ROI-based analyses focused on three most commonly-studied, WM-related brain areas: early visual cortex (EVC), intraparietal sulcus (IPS) in parietal cortex, and superior precentral sulcus (sPCS) in frontal cortex (Ester et al., 2015; Hallenbeck et al., 2021; Yu & Shim, 2017). We created anatomical ROI masks based on the probabilistic atlas by Wang and colleagues (Wang et al., 2015). EVC (merging bilateral V1, V2, and V3), IPS (merging bilateral IPS0-5), and sPCS masks were generated by warping masks from the probabilistic atlas to individuals’ anatomical image in their native space. In order to generate functional ROI masks, we then performed general linear models (GLMs) to quantify task-related univariate activity changes in each voxel. Task events were modeled using boxcar functions convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function (durations of event epochs for sample, post retro-cue delay, memory delay, and response were 2.5 s, 2 s, 8.5 s, and 2 s, respectively). Six nuisance regressors were also included to account for head motion artifacts in the six dimensions of rigid body motion. Functional EVC mask was defined by the 500 most active voxels during sample display. Functional IPS and sPCS masks were defined by the 500 most active voxels during memory delay.
MRI Data Analyses
Multivariate Inverted Encoding Modeling (IEM)
Neural representations of orientations were reconstructed using inverted encoding modeling (IEM) (Brouwer & Heeger, 2009, 2011; Ester et al., 2015; Rademaker et al., 2019; Yu & Shim, 2017) with custom MATLAB scripts on individuals’ BOLD activation patterns in the three ROIs. IEM provides an estimate of population-level reconstructions of stimulus-specific information. The general procedure for IEM includes using training data to train model weights and then applying weights to testing data to obtain reconstructed channel responses. For the main analyses, we used trials from all conditions to train and to test IEM in order to avoid potential biases from a specific task condition. Results for categorization task were averaged across rules for Experiment 1. Training and testing were performed for each TR separately. As a control, IEMs were also estimated for each condition separately (within-condition IEM). Training and testing underwent a leave-one-run-out cross-validation procedure, in which each run was taken out as the testing run, and the rest of the data served as the training run. This procedure was iterated until all runs had served as training and testing runs. Results from all cross-validated folds were averaged. Detailed computations for each fold were elucidated below:
We first modeled responses of voxels into nine equidistant orientation channels (initial channels were 1°, 21°, 41°, 61°, 81°, 101°, 121°, 141°, 161°), characterizing voxel selectivity for orientations. At each channel, the modeled orientation tuning curve was a half-wave-rectified and squared sinusoid raised to eighth power, defined as the function below (c was the center of the channel):
Population-level tuning responses of voxels was described using the function:
B1 was the training dataset from our fMRI data (ν voxels × n trials). C1 represented the hypothesized channel responses (k channels × n trials) which were modulated by W, a weight matrix (ν voxels × k channels).
The least-squared estimates of the weight matrix (Ŵ) was computed using linear regression:
The weight matrix was then applied on the test dataset to reconstruct estimated channel responses (Ĉ2):
The analysis above were repeated for 20 times in step of 1° using leave-one-run-out cross-validation so that the nine channel centers covered all 180 orientations (Rademaker et al., 2019; Yu & Postle, 2021). All channel responses were combined to create responses for all 180 orientation channels. For statistical comparisons and for visualization, all channel responses were shifted to a common center of 0° (true orientation of trials). The responses from all trials were averaged to obtain reconstructed orientation representations for the test datasets.
To quantify the strength of each IEM reconstruction, we calculated reconstruction fidelity of channel responses. First, all channel responses were projected to the vector at the true orientation. Then the reconstruction fidelity was calculated as the mean of all projected vectors (Rademaker et al., 2019). A larger fidelity value indicates a stronger positive representation of orientation.
Multivariate Pattern Analysis (MVPA)
Besides IEM, we tracked neural representations of stimulus and of category using linear Support Vector Machine (SVM) decoders. All decoding analyses were performed using the templateSVM and fitcecoc functions in MATLAB.
Decoding of stimulus was performed for every TR. We divided the thirty orientations into four bins of 45° each, two cardinal bins centered at 90° or 180° and two oblique bins centered at 45° or 135°. We then performed two two-way classifications, one trained and tested on cardinal bins, and the other trained and tested on oblique bins. We trained and tested decoders separately for each condition using the same leave-one-run-out cross-validation procedure as in IEM analyses. To avoid biases in model training, we randomly balanced the trial numbers for each bin in the training set. Decoding accuracies were then computed by averaging performance of cardinal and oblique classifiers. For the categorization task, we averaged accuracies across rules.
Decoding of category information for Experiment 1 was performed under each rule (90 trials for each rule) using a leave-one-trial-out cross-validation procedure (see next paragraph for details), and the decoding accuracies were then averaged across rules. Since Experiment 2 adopted a fixed rule with 180 trials in the categorization task, we randomly divided categorization trials into two halves with 90 trials each, and decoded category information for Experiment 2 using identical procedures as for Experiment 1.
Because closer orientations are more similar to each other inherently, orientations per se could contain categorical information by visual similarity. Thus, to isolate the influence of stimulus on category, in addition to the decoder using true category labels, we trained an opposite category decoder using category labels based on the opposite rule. If the two-way classification on categories only captured stimulus information, then true category and opposite category decoding should have had comparable performance. If abstract category information existed beyond stimulus information, then true category decoder should have outperformed the opposite category decoder. Thus, an abstract category index was calculated by subtracting opposite category decoding accuracy from true category decoding accuracy (i.e., chance level = 0). Since the opposite category decoding used re-assigned labels, to eliminate imbalance in trial number between true and opposite categories, we used a leave-one-trial-out cross-validation procedure for true category and opposite category decoders. Decoding for Experiment 1 was performed separately for each rule and were then averaged. Decoding for Experiment 2 was performed separately for randomly divided halves and averaged.
Recurrent Neural Network Simulations
RNN architecture
The network model was built following the details in previous work (Masse et al., 2019), and implemented in TensorFlow (version: Nvidia-tensorflow 1.15.0) (Abadi et al., 2016). The general network architecture consisted of three layers of artificial units: the input, hidden, and output layers. The input layer contains units served to present various task-related signals corresponding to those in the fMRI paradigm, including orientations, retro-cues, task cues. In order to simulate neural activity patterns in hierarchically connected brain regions (EVC, IPS and sPCS), we separated the hidden layer into three modules, each containing 200 recurrent units with short-term synaptic plasticity (STSP). Within each module, units were further divided into 80% excitatory and 20% inhibitory following Dale’s principle. Similar to previous work (Zhou et al., 2021), modularity was achieved by constraining the recurrent connectivity in the hidden layer. Specifically, only posterior module’s (module 1) excitatory units received inputs from the input layer and only anterior module’s (module 3) excitatory units projected to the output units. Between-module connections were culled so that only 50% of a module’s excitatory units were randomly connected to their counterparts in the neighboring module(s), and vice versa (feedforward and feedback connections). Connections among inhibitory units remained strictly within-module in accordance with the observation that inhibitory connections in cortex are largely local. Thus, posterior, middle and anterior modules were intended to simulate the three interconnected ROIs we used in the fMRI analyses that posited differently at the processing hierarchy. We specifically manipulated the output demand to investigate whether it would alter similarity of the results to the fMRI observations. To this end, one type of network architecture (RNN1) implemented a two-unit output layer with each unit corresponding to one of two possible response options, presented through the input units before the test period; In contrast, the other type of RNN architecture (RNN2) had additional units in the output layer, creating a demand for preserving the original stimulus information alongside categorical representations.
Task simulation
Orientations were simulated as Gaussian signals from 15 orientation-tuned units in the input layer distributed equally across 0-180 degrees, forming a ring of receptive field. The magnitude of an orientation-tuned input unit represented the closeness of its preferred orientation to the input angle. Stimulus values were selected from an array of 20 orientations evenly spanned from [0 to 180) degrees. The sequentially-presented stimuli were presented through the same receptive field, followed by retro-cue and task cue indicated through the separate input units. For RNN1, before the test period when the network was required to make a choice, two response options were presented sequentially through the same input receptive field. The selection of the options varied slightly between the maintenance and categorization tasks: in maintenance, one orientation was always the cued sample while the other was randomly chosen from all other possible angles. In categorization, one option was taken from the same category as the cued sample but not necessarily the exact angle, while the other option was randomly chosen from orientations belonging to the other category. The network output (0,1) or (1,0) in the output units to report its choice. In comparison, RNN2 output (0,1) or (1,0) to report the category to which the cued orientation belonged in the categorization task, or (0,0) in the maintenance task. Importantly, the model also needed to report the cued orientation itself through a receptive field consisting of 15 orientation-tuned units in the output layer.
Training parameters and procedure
The hyperparameters and procedure for training the models were consistent with those detailed in previous work (Masse et al., 2019), with the following exceptions: standard deviations of input and recurrent noise were set to 0.01 as our tasks were much harder to train compared to those used in the reference study (especially networks were trained on both tasks simultaneously). Lowering the noise level may provide an edge for the models to successfully learn to perform the tasks. In a similar vein, we also expanded the number of hidden units to 600 and number of training iteration to a maximum of 10000. Additionally, spike penalty was set to 0 for both RNN models to remove constraints on neuronal activity.
We trained 20 models for each type of RNN and results were obtained by averaging over all of them (for single-rule RNN, 10 models for each categorization rule). The goal of the training process was to minimize the mean square error between the model outputs and correct outputs during the test period via back propagation (with a 50 ms grace period at onset when model output was not taken into account in calculating error). Training was conducted in a block-interleaved fashion in which each gradient batch consisted of 300 maintenance, 300 categorization Rule A and 300 categorization Rule B trials, with the task block order randomized (for single rule RNN, each batch consisted of 300 maintenance and 300 categorization trials). Training would automatically stop if the model achieved 90% accuracy in the last training batch on all tasks. For RNN2, the accuracies for category and stimulus outputs were calculated separately to ensure precision of the stimulus outputs.
Population decoding
We measured the strength of stimulus and category representations through training SVMs on time-resolved neuronal activity. Activities were obtained by feeding new batches of tasks into the already-successfully trained networks after freezing all connection weights to prevent further changes to the models’ behaviors. The intrinsic noise for the recurrent layer was also set to 0 for decoding analyses. To ensure accurate decoding results, we sampled large number of trials (900 trials for each condition) and implemented a 5-fold cross-validation procedure in which 80% of trials were used as training set and the remaining 20% as testing set in each fold. Decoders were trained separately for each module and time point.
Statistical Testing
Participants’ behavioral performance for the main task was assessed using accuracy and reaction time. Paired t-test was conducted for the two task types (maintenance & categorization) to evaluate differences between conditions.
Statistical significance was evaluated via a sign-flip permutation procedure for all other analyses. For example, to characterize the significance of IEM fidelity, we computed the p-value by comparing the true mean fidelity of our sample with a null distribution reflecting no IEM fidelity. The null distribution was created by randomly assigning 1 or -1 to fidelity scores of our sample and then averaging the sign-flipped samples for 10,000 times, resulting in a null distribution of 10,000 fidelity scores. To characterize the difference of IEM fidelity between tasks, we sign-flipped the fidelity sample for each condition and then averaged the difference for 10,000 times. The p-value was calculated by comparing the true mean difference with the generated null distribution of difference. P-values were corrected using FDR across ROIs, time (TRs), and tasks for all analyses unless specified. An early (5-10 s; 6th-11th TR) and late (11-16 s; 12th-17th TR) task epoch was also defined to facilitate comparisons between ROIs and experiments when needed.
For RNN decoding results, we adopted a cluster-based permutation approach (using MNE-Python (Gramfort et al., 2013) function permutation_cluster_1samp_test to accommodate the large number of time points to be corrected for) to determine statistical significance of the time course, for stimulus decoding accuracies in maintenance/categorization, difference between stimulus decoding accuracies between tasks, and abstract category decoding accuracy in categorization task. Moreover, we also pooled decoding accuracies across a critical task period (50-75 time points during delay) to produce summary statistics aligning with what was reported in the fMRI results. Average decoding results were corrected using the FDR method.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Dr. Mu-ming Poo for valuable comments on an earlier version of the manuscript, and Dr. Tianming Yang for helpful discussions. This work was supported by the Ministry of Science and Technology of China (STI2030-Major Projects 2021ZD0203701, 2021ZD0204202), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (32271089), CAS Project for Young Scientists in Basic Research (YSBR-071), and Shanghai Pujiang Program (22PJ1414400) to Q.Y.
References
- 1.TensorFlow: A System for Large-Scale Machine Learning12th USENIX Symposium on Operating Systems Design and Implementation :265–283
- 2.Working memory: looking back and looking forwardNat Rev Neurosci 4:829–839https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201
- 3.Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro-caudal organization of the frontal lobesTrends Cogn Sci 12:193–200https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.004
- 4.The dimensionality of neural representations for controlCurr Opin Behav Sci 38:20–28https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.07.002
- 5.Frontal cortex and the discovery of abstract action rulesNeuron 66:315–326https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.025
- 6.Decoding the content of visual short-term memory under distraction in occipital and parietal areasNat Neurosci 19:150–157https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4174
- 7.The Psychophysics ToolboxSpat Vis 10:433–436
- 8.Gradual progression from sensory to task-related processing in cerebral cortexProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115:E7202–E7211https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1717075115
- 9.Decoding and reconstructing color from responses in human visual cortexJ Neurosci 29:13992–14003https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3577-09.2009
- 10.Cross-orientation suppression in human visual cortexJ Neurophysiol 106:2108–2119https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00540.2011
- 11.Decoding the contents of visual short-term memory from human visual and parietal cortexJ Neurosci 32:12983–12989https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0184-12.2012
- 12.Cortical specialization for attended versus unattended working memoryNat Neurosci 21:494–496https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0094-4
- 13.AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimagesComput Biomed Res 29:162–173
- 14.Software tools for analysis and visualization of FMRI DataNMR in Biomedicine 10:171–178
- 15.The cognitive neuroscience of working memoryAnnu Rev Psychol 66:115–142https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031
- 16.Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: an event-related fMRI studyBrain Cogn 41:66–86https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1999.1096
- 17.Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studiesExp Brain Res 133:3–11https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210000395
- 18.Distributed patterns of activity in sensory cortex reflect the precision of multiple items maintained in visual short-term memoryJ Neurosci 33:6516–6523https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5732-12.2013
- 19.Meta-control: From psychology to computational neuroscienceCogn Affect Behav Neurosci 21:447–452https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-021-00919-4
- 20.A neural measure of precision in visual working memoryJ Cogn Neurosci 25:754–761https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00357
- 21.Parietal and Frontal Cortex Encode Stimulus-Specific Mnemonic Representations during Visual Working MemoryNeuron 87:893–905https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.013
- 22.Categorical Biases in Human Occipitoparietal CortexJ Neurosci 40:917–931https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2700-19.2019
- 23.Orthogonal representations for robust context-dependent task performance in brains and neural networksNeuron 110:1258–1270https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2022.01.005
- 24.Experience-dependent representation of visual categories in parietal cortexNature 443:85–88https://doi.org/10.1038/nature05078
- 25.Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortexScience 291:312–316https://doi.org/10.1126/science.291.5502.312
- 26.Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey’s dorsolateral prefrontal cortexJ Neurophysiol 61:331–349https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1989.61.2.331
- 27.Why neurons mix: high dimensionality for higher cognitionCurr Opin Neurobiol 37:66–74https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2016.01.010
- 28.Neuron activity related to short-term memoryScience 173:652–654
- 29.Parietal-Occipital Interactions Underlying Control- and Representation-Related Processes in Working Memory for Nonspatial Visual FeaturesJ Neurosci 38:4357–4366https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2747-17.2018
- 30.MEG and EEG data analysis with MNE-PythonFront Neurosci 7https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00267
- 31.Working memory representations in visual cortex mediate distraction effectsNat Commun 12https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-24973-1
- 32.Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areasNature 458:632–635https://doi.org/10.1038/nature07832
- 33.Flexible utilization of spatial- and motor-based codes for the storage of visuo-spatial informationElife 11https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.75688
- 34.Spatiotemporal dynamics of self-generated imagery reveal a reverse cortical hierarchy from cue-induced imageryCell Rep 42https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2023.113242
- 35.Low-dimensional encoding of decisions in parietal cortex reflects long-term training historyNat Commun 14https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-36554-5
- 36.Sustained Activity Encoding Working Memories: Not Fully DistributedTrends Neurosci 40:328–346https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2017.04.004
- 37.Goal-dependent dissociation of visual and prefrontal cortices during working memoryNat Neurosci 16:997–999https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3452
- 38.Neural Representations in Visual and Parietal Cortex Differentiate between Imagined, Perceived, and Illusory ExperiencesJ Neurosci 43:6508–6524https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0592-23.2023
- 39.Inverted Encoding Models of Human Population Response Conflate Noise and Neural Tuning WidthJ Neurosci 38:398–408https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2453-17.2017
- 40.Flexible coding of visual working memory representations during distractionJ Neurosci https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3061-17.2018
- 41.Categorical judgments do not modify sensory representations in working memoryPLoS Comput Biol 17https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008968
- 42.Circuit mechanisms for the maintenance and manipulation of information in working memoryNat Neurosci 22:1159–1167https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0414-3
- 43.Task dependence of visual and category representations in prefrontal and inferior temporal corticesJ Neurosci 34:16065–16075https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1660-14.2014
- 44.An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex functionAnnu Rev Neurosci 24:167–202https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
- 45.Long-term learning transforms prefrontal cortex representations during working memoryNeuron 110:3805–3819https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2022.09.019
- 46.Abstract Neural Representations of Category Membership beyond Information Coding Stimulus or ResponseJ Cogn Neurosci :1–17https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01651
- 47.Rationalizing constraints on the capacity for cognitive controlTrends Cogn Sci 25:757–775https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.06.001
- 48.The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into moviesSpat Vis 10:437–442
- 49.Coexisting representations of sensory and mnemonic information in human visual cortexNat Neurosci 22:1336–1344https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0428-x
- 50.The Relationship between Working Memory Storage and Elevated Activity as Measured with Functional Magnetic Resonance ImagingJournal of Neuroscience 32:12990–12998https://doi.org/10.1523/Jneurosci.1892-12.2012
- 51.Stimulus-specific delay activity in human primary visual cortexPsychol Sci 20:207–214https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02276.x
- 52.Inverted Encoding Models Assay Population-Level Stimulus Representations, Not Single-Unit Neural TuningeNeuro 5https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0098-18.2018
- 53.Attention modulates spatial priority maps in the human occipital, parietal and frontal corticesNat Neurosci 16:1879–1887https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3574
- 54.Probabilistic Maps of Visual Topography in Human CortexCereb Cortex 25:3911–3931https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu277
- 55.50 years of mnemonic persistent activity: quo vadis?Trends Neurosci 44:888–902https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2021.09.001
- 56.The Neural Codes Underlying Internally Generated Representations in Visual Working MemoryJ Cogn Neurosci :1–16https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01702
- 57.Occipital, parietal, and frontal cortices selectively maintain task-relevant features of multi-feature objects in visual working memoryNeuroimage 157:97–107https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.055
- 58.Temporal-Order-Based Attentional Priority Modulates Mnemonic Representations in Parietal and Frontal CorticesCereb Cortex 29:3182–3192https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy184
- 59.Distributed functions of prefrontal and parietal cortices during sequential categorical decisionsElife 10https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.58782
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Shao et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 295
- downloads
- 24
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.