Abstract
Direct electrical stimulation of the human brain has been used for numerous clinical and scientific applications. Previously, we demonstrated that intracranial theta burst stimulation (TBS) of the basolateral amygdala (BLA) can enhance declarative memory, likely by modulating hippocampal-dependent memory consolidation. At present, however, little is known about how intracranial stimulation affects activity at the microscale. In this study, we recorded intracranial EEG data from a cohort of patients with medically refractory epilepsy as they completed a visual recognition memory task. During the memory task, brief trains of TBS were delivered to the BLA. Using simultaneous microelectrode recordings, we isolated neurons in the hippocampus, amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex and tested whether stimulation enhanced or suppressed firing rates. Additionally, we characterized the properties of modulated neurons, patterns of firing rate coactivity, and the extent to which modulation affected memory task performance. We observed a subset of neurons (∼30%) whose firing rate was modulated by TBS, exhibiting highly heterogeneous responses with respect to onset latency, duration, and direction of effect. Notably, location and baseline activity predicted which neurons were most susceptible to modulation, although the impact of this neuronal modulation on memory remains unclear. These findings advance our limited understanding of how focal electrical fields influence neuronal firing at the single-cell level and motivate future neuromodulatory therapies that aim to recapitulate specific patterns of activity implicated in cognition and memory.
Highlights
Individual neurons in the human brain were responsive to theta burst stimulation
Basolateral amygdala stimulation preferentially modulated neurons in the hippocampus, orbitofrontal cortex, and amygdala
Neurons modulated by stimulation tended to have greater baseline firing rates
Duration, onset, and valence of neuronal modulation were heterogeneous
Introduction
The amygdala is a highly connected cluster of nuclei with input from multiple sensory modalities and vast projections to distributed cortical and subcortical regions involved in autonomic regulation and cognition (Kim et al., 2011; McDonald and Mott, 2017; Roy et al., 2009; Stein et al., 2007). Numerous studies have described the amygdala’s capacity to facilitate the encoding of long-lasting emotional memories (Dolcos et al., 2004; Hermans et al., 2014; LaBar and Cabeza, 2006; Phelps, 2006, 2004; Phelps and LeDoux, 2005; Qasim et al., 2023; Richardson et al., 2004; Roozendaal et al., 2009; Schwabe et al., 2013; Zheng et al., 2017). Specific oscillatory dynamics in the amygdalohippocampal circuit are increasingly understood to be essential in prioritizing the encoding of these salient memories (Qasim et al., 2023; Zheng et al., 2019, 2017).
Recently, direct electrical stimulation of the basolateral complex of the amygdala (BLA) in humans has revealed a more generalized ability to enhance declarative memory irrespective of the emotional valence (Inman et al., 2018), likely by promoting synaptic plasticity-related processes underlying memory consolidation in the hippocampus and medial temporal lobe (McGaugh, 2013, 2004; McGaugh et al., 2002; Roesler et al., 2021). These pro-memory effects were achieved with rhythmic theta-burst stimulation (TBS), which is known to induce long-term potentiation (LTP), a key mechanism in memory formation (Larson and Munkácsy, 2015). Emerging evidence suggests that intracranial TBS may also enhance memory specificity (Titiz et al., 2017), evoke theta-frequency oscillations (Solomon et al., 2021), and facilitate short-term plasticity in local field potential recordings (Herrero et al., 2021; Huang et al., 2024). However, the extent to which exogenous TBS modulates activity at the single-cell level and whether this modulation is associated with memory performance remains poorly understood.
Here, we address this knowledge gap by conducting simultaneous microelectrode recordings from prefrontal and medial temporal structures during a memory task in which intracranial TBS was applied to the BLA. To characterize neuronal modulation, we contrasted the trial-averaged, peri-stimulation firing rates across stim and no-stim conditions; neurons were further classified based on their location, baseline activity, and direction of effect (enhancement vs. suppression). We predicted that intracranial TBS of the BLA would modulate spiking activity within highly connected regions (e.g., hippocampus) and improve memory task performance.
Methods
Participants
We report results from a cohort of 23 patients with medically refractory epilepsy who underwent stereoelectroencephalography to localize epileptogenic foci (74% female, 19– 66 years old). All patients were age 18+ and able to provide informed consent. No exclusion was made concerning a patient’s sex, gender, race, ethnicity, or socioeconomic status. Surgeries were performed at the University of Utah in Salt Lake City, UT (n = 10) and Barnes-Jewish Hospital in St. Louis, MO (n = 13). Patients were monitored continuously by a clinical team during their post-operative hospital course. Each patient signed a written informed consent form before participation in the research study; all study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Utah (IRB 00144045, IRB 00114691) and Washington University (IRB 202104033).
Electrode Placement and Localization
Numbers and trajectories of stereoelectroencephalography electrode placements were determined case-by-case and solely derived from clinical considerations during a multidisciplinary case conference without reference to this research program. Each patient was implanted with clinical macroelectrodes and 1–3 Behnke-Fried depth electrodes (Ad-Tech Medical Instrument Corporation, Oak Creek, WI), which contained both macro- and microelectrode contacts (eight 40 µm diameter microwires and one unshielded reference wire) for recording local field potentials and extracellular action potentials, respectively (Figure 1A). To localize electrodes, we leveraged the open-access Localize Electrodes Graphical User Interface (LeGUI) (Davis et al., 2021) software developed by our group, which performs coregistration of pre-operative MRI and post-operative CT sequences, image processing, normalization to standard anatomical templates, and automated electrode detection.

Microelectrode locations, unit counts, and experimental design.
(A) Behnke Fried-style macro/micro depth electrode (left) and microelectrode bundle locations projected in MNI space (right). (B) The proportion of units recorded from each brain area (left) and the proportion of units that met the criteria for inclusion in analyses (average pre-trial baseline firing rate ≥ 0.1 Hz) (right). (C) Counts of total (grey) and included (colored) units within each region. (D) Intracranial recording and stimulation took place in the context of a two-phase (encoding, retrieval) visual recognition memory task. A series of neutral valence images were shown (3 s), half of which were followed by direct electrical stimulation (1 s). Retrieval memory was tested during a self-paced task ∼24 hours later. (E) Simulated theta-burst stimulation trace (left) and individual stimulation pulse (right); charge-balanced, bipolar, biphasic rectangular pulses were delivered over a 1 s period. HIP = hippocampus (coral), OFC = orbitofrontal cortex (yellow), AMY = amygdala (blue), ACC = anterior cingulate cortex (purple).
Intracranial Electrophysiology
Neurophysiological data were recorded at both hospitals using a neural signal processor (Blackrock Microsystems, Salt Lake City, UT; Nihon Koden USA, Irvine, CA) sampling at 30 kHz. Microelectrode contacts were locally referenced to a low-impedance microwire near the recording wires. Macroelectrode contacts were referenced to an intracranial contact located within the white matter with minimal activity, per recommended practices (Mercier et al., 2022).
Experimental Design
Patients completed a visual recognition memory task previously employed by our group to characterize the effects of basolateral amygdala stimulation upon memory consolidation (Inman et al., 2018). The memory task consisted of an encoding session, during which a series of neutral valence images were presented, and a self-paced retrieval session ∼24 hours post-encoding wherein patients were asked to indicate whether each image onscreen was old (previously shown) or new (unseen) (Figure 2A). Data were aggregated across four highly similar experimental paradigms with minor differences in the content of visual stimuli, number of trials, stimulation parameters, and testing intervals. Each encoding session consisted of 160 or 320 trials wherein an image was presented on screen for 3 s, followed by a ∼6 s interstimulus interval (fixation cross on screen).

Example raster plots depicting heterogeneous responses to stimulation.
(A) Representative example of modulation during stimulation. The highpass-filtered, trial-averaged LFP from the corresponding microwire is shown (top) above the spike raster for an example unit located in the hippocampus (middle); the grey shaded region depicts the duration of stimulation with onset at t = 0. The average firing rate across trials was estimated by convolving the binned spike counts (100 ms bins) with a Gaussian kernel (bottom). (B) The difference in the number of spikes in the 1 s peri-stimulation epochs for each trial is shown (top). We subsequently performed a Wilcoxon signed-rank test on the during- and post-stimulation spike counts for each trial vs. the pre-trial baseline and compared the empirical test statistic against a null distribution generated by shuffling the epoch labels 1,000 times (bottom); the grey-shaded region represents the distribution containing 95% of observed values. (C) Some units (left, left-middle) exhibited increased firing rates, whereas others (right-middle, right) had their firing suppressed. The temporal dynamics of the firing rate modulation (e.g., onset, duration) were highly variable across units. The averaged waveform for each of the visualized units is shown below its corresponding peri-stimulation raster plot (WFs = waveforms); the shaded region represents standard deviation across waveforms.
Intracranial Theta Burst Stimulation
We delivered direct electrical stimulation to the basolateral amygdala during half of the trials in the encoding phase of each experimental session. Stimulation pulses were delivered immediately once the image was removed from the screen and in a patterned rhythm designed to entrain endogenous theta-gamma oscillatory interactions (i.e., theta-burst stimulation, TBS) (Hanslmayr et al., 2016; Suppa et al., 2016). Specifically, we administered charge-balanced, bipolar, 1 mA, biphasic rectangular pulses over a 1 s period with a 50% duty cycle. Stimulation pulses were delivered at a rate of 50 Hz and nested within eight equally-spaced bursts (∼8 Hz) (Figure 2B–C). A subset of experiments (n = 7) used a lower current (0.5 mA) with variable pulse frequencies across trials (33 Hz, 50 Hz, 80 Hz).
Spike Detection and Sorting
Microelectrode data were first filtered between 250–500 Hz with a zero-phase lag bandpass filter and re-thresholded offline at -3.5 times the root mean square of the signal to identify spike waveforms. Units were isolated during a semi-automated process within Offline Sorter (Plexon Inc, Dallas, TX) by applying the T-distribution expectation maximization method on the first three principal components of the detected waveforms (initial parameters: degrees of freedom multiplier = 4, initial number of units = 3) (Shoham et al., 2003). Finally, the waveform shapes, interspike interval distribution, consistency of firing, and isolation from other waveform clusters were manually inspected for further curation and removal of spurious, non-physiological threshold crossings that could represent stimulation artifacts.
Single Unit Quality Metrics
We calculated several distinct metrics to characterize detected units’ properties and assess the quality of our spike sorting (Figure S1): the number of units detected per microelectrode bundle, the mean firing rate (Hz) for each unit, the percentage of interspike intervals < 3 ms, the coefficient of variation across each unit’s spike train, the average presence ratio of firing in 1s bins (proportion of bins which contain ≥ 1 spike), the ratio between the peak amplitude of the averaged waveform and its standard deviation, and the mean signal-to-noise ratio of the averaged waveform.
Peri-Stimulation Firing Rate Analyses
We first created peri-stimulation epochs (1 s pre-trial ISI, 1 s after image onset, 1 s during stimulation/after image offset, 1 s post-stimulation), with t = 0 representing stimulation onset and the moment at which the image was removed from the screen (Figure 2A); identical epochs were created for the image-only (no-stimulation) trials. Units with a trial-averaged baseline (pre-trial ISI) firing rate of < 0.1 Hz were excluded from subsequent analyses because low firing rates limited the ability to detect modulation robustly. Units were designated as “modulated” if either the during- or post-stimulation firing rate contrast was significant following permutation testing (described in Statistical Analyses). An additional contrast of pre-trial ISI vs. image onset was performed to evaluate the sensitivity of neurons to task stimuli (i.e., image presentation).
Firing Rate Control Analyses
We performed a sensitivity analysis by systematically varying the baseline firing rate threshold used to exclude units from modulation analyses. The threshold for inclusion of units was varied from 0–3 Hz (0.1 Hz step size), and the firing rate analyses were repeated to quantify the proportion of units meeting inclusion criteria and the proportion of units designated as modulated (Figure S5A). Next, we performed a dropout analysis wherein segments of data near the onset of a stimulation burst were removed from the during-stimulation epoch (an identical segment was also removed from the pre-trial ISI and post-stimulation epochs). To this end, we removed a window of data starting at the onset of each burst spanning 0–60 ms (5 ms step size, eight bursts in train) and recomputed the proportion of units meeting inclusion criteria and the proportion of units designated as modulated (Figure S5B).
Population Analyses
To analyze pseudo-population activity, we performed a linear dimensionality reduction with PCA on a matrix of the z-scored trial-averaged firing rates of all neurons recorded across patients (sklearn.decomposition.PCA). Doing so allowed us to qualitatively examine the temporal dynamics of the dominant modes of neuron coactivity in a low-dimensional subspace, separated by experimental condition (stim vs. no-stim), region, and laterality relative to stimulation. The firing rate matrices for each condition were concatenated prior to PCA to facilitate direct comparison among the principal components; for simplicity, only the first three principal components are visualized.
Statistical Approach
All statistical analyses were conducted using custom Python scripts and established statistical libraries (i.e., Scikit-learn (Pedregosa et al., 2012), Scipy (Virtanen et al., 2020), Statsmodels (Seabold and Perktold, 2010)). We performed two separate Wilcoxon signed-rank tests (scipy.stats.wilcoxon) across trials on the during- and post-stimulation spike counts relative to their corresponding pre-trail baseline spike counts. To control for false positives, we compared the empirical test statistic against a null distribution generated from shuffling pre/during/post epoch labels (n = 1000 permutations) (Figure 2B). An identical analysis was also performed on the no-stimulation (image-only) trials.
To test for differences in the proportion of modulated units (across conditions, regions, stimulation parameters, and behavioral outcomes), we performed a series of one- and two-sided Fisher’s exact tests (scipy.stats.fisher_exact) (Figure 3A–B, Figure 4C, Figure S4). Next, we used Mann-Whitney U tests to contrast baseline firing rates among modulated vs. unaffected units (scipy.stats.mannwhitneyu) (Figure 3C). Behavioral performance during the memory task was calculated using d-prime (d’), defined as the difference in an individual’s z-scored hit rate and false alarm rate. Observed changes in recognition memory were split into two categories using a d’ difference threshold of ± 0.2: responder (Δd’ < -.2 or Δd’ > +.2) or non-responder (-0.2 ≤ Δd’ ≤ 0.2). The threshold of ± 0.2 was chosen based on the defined range of a “small effect” for Cohen’s d, which bears conceptual similarity to d’. To test the hypothesis that stimulation affected behavioral performance, we used a linear mixed effects model with d’ score as the dependent variable, condition and experiment as fixed effects, and session as a random effect (statsmodels.regression.mixed_linear_model.MixedLM) (Figure 4A); an additional test for differences among hit rates (percent of previously seen images correctly identified) was implemented using a paired-samples t-test (scipy.stats.ttest_rel) (Figure 4B).

Characterization of modulation in neuronal firing rate.
(A) Percent of modulated units observed across trials separated by stim (purple) vs. no-stim conditions (orange). (B) Percent of modulated units as a function of recording region. (C) Comparison of baseline firing rate in units separated by condition (stim vs. no-stim) and outcome (NS = not significant, Mod = modulated). (D) Venn diagram depicting the shared and independent proportions of units modulated by image onset (Image) and the two experimental conditions (stim vs. no-stim). (E) Scatterplot of pre-stimulation firing rate relative to the firing rate during the two contrast windows (during, post) for the stim (left) and no-stim (right) conditions. Modulated units are highlighted in purple (stim) or orange (no-stim). (F) Temporal dynamics of pseudo-population coactivity within each condition, represented by the first three principal components of the trial-averaged firing rates. The grey-shaded region depicts the duration of stimulation with onset at t = 0. Images were presented on screen for 3 s, with onset at t = -3. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001, NS = not significant.

Summary of behavioral performance during memory task.
(A) Memory performance for each session is quantified using d’ (left); grey lines connect d’ scores across conditions for an individual session. Boxplot of the observed difference in d’ scores across conditions (right). (B) Hit rate (percent of old images correctly recognized) and false alarm rate (percent of new images incorrectly labeled as old) across conditions.
Results
We recorded single-unit activity from 23 patients (n = 30 sessions) with medically refractory epilepsy as they completed a visual recognition memory task. During the encoding session of the experiment, each patient received either 80 or 160 trials of bipolar intracranial TBS to a contiguous pair of macroelectrode contacts in the BLA. An equal number of “no-stimulation” trials were randomly interspersed to evaluate the effect of stimulation on memory performance and control for neuronal modulation resulting from experimental stimuli (e.g., image presentation). In total, we isolated 203 putative neurons from 68 bundles of 8 microwires each, distributed among recording sites in the hippocampus (HIP, n = 95 units), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC, n = 44), amygdala (AMY, n = 39), and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC, n = 25) (Figure 1; see also Figure S1 and Figure S2); a subset of these units (n = 47, 23.2%) was excluded from subsequent analyses because low baseline firing rates (< 0.1 Hz) limited the ability to robustly detect modulation.
Theta Burst Stimulation of the BLA Modulates Widely Distributed Populations of Neurons
We hypothesized that BLA stimulation would modulate neuronal activity in the sampled regions, given the amygdala’s well-established connectivity to the HIP, OFC, and ACC (Roy et al., 2009). To test this hypothesis, we quantified spike counts across trials within peri-stimulation epochs (1 s pre-trial ISI, 1 s after image onset, 1 s during stimulation/after image offset, and 1 s post-stimulation) and used Wilcoxon signed-rank tests to compare the spike counts against a null distribution generated by shuffling epoch labels. We performed two firing rate contrasts across trials (pre-trial ISI vs. during stimulation, pre-trial ISI vs. post-stimulation) within two distinct conditions (stim, no-stim); an additional contrast of the pre-trial ISI vs. image onset epochs was included to evaluate the sensitivity of neurons to task image presentations (Figure 2A).
BLA TBS modulated firing rates in 30.1% of all recorded units, a significantly higher proportion than the 15.4% responsive to no-stim (image only) trials (one-sided Fisher’s exact test, OR = 2.37, p < 0.001; Figure 3A). Across all regions sampled, we observed units modulated by the stim and no-stim conditions. Units in HIP (OR = 2.07, p = 0.044), OFC (OR = 5.09, p = 0.040), and AMY (OR = 3.33, p = 0.042) were most sensitive to stimulation; we did not observe a difference in the proportions of units within the ACC responsive to the stim vs. no-stim conditions (OR = 1.00, p = 0.661; Figure 3B). Only 9.0% of units responded to both the stim and no-stim conditions, despite approximately half of the stim-modulated units (representing 14.7% of all units) exhibiting a change in firing rate associated with image onset (Figure 3D). This result suggests that the units modulated by stimulation are largely distinct from those responsive to image offset during trials without stimulation.
Because neuronal firing properties vary across cell types (Barthó et al., 2004; Keller et al., 2010; Peyrache et al., 2012; Quyen et al., 2008), we also tested whether baseline (pre-trial ISI) firing rates predicted a unit’s response to stimulation, suggestive of selective engagement of specific neuron populations. Stimulation-modulated units exhibited significantly higher baseline firing rates compared to unaffected units (U(NStim, Mod = 47, NStim, NS = 109) = 3450.50, p < 0.001). No difference in baseline firing rate was observed among units modulated in the no-stim condition, compared to those that were unaffected (U(NNo-Stim, Mod = 24, NNo-Stim, NS = 132) = 1964.00, p = 0.062) (Figure 3C). The median (Q1, Q3) baseline firing rates for modulated units in the stim and no-stim conditions were 1.77 Hz (0.95 Hz, 5.39 Hz) and 1.53 Hz (0.72 Hz, 5.41 Hz), respectively.
Our exploratory analyses of pseudo-population activity revealed interesting temporal dynamics associated with image presentation and the delivery of stimulation. More specifically, we observed variation among the first three principal components across both stim and no-stim trials associated with image presentation (t = -3 to t = 0) and robust shifts in coactivity modes associated with stimulation (t = 0 to t = 1) (Figure 3F). Further characterization of these dynamics suggests that TBS of the BLA primarily resulted in transient changes to firing rate coactivity within hippocampal neurons and was present regardless of the neuron’s laterality to stimulation (Figure S3A–B).
In a subset of experimental sessions (n = 7), we explored the effects of different stimulation parameters on neuronal modulation within an experimental session; more specifically, we employed a lower stimulation amplitude (0.5 mA vs. 1.0 mA) and varied pulse frequency (33 Hz vs. 50 Hz and 33 Hz vs. 80 Hz). Neither the amplitude of stimulation (OR = 1.69, p = 0.302, n = 30) nor pulse frequency (33 vs. 80 Hz; OR = 0.00, p = 1.000, n = 1; 50 vs. 80 Hz; OR = 1.40, p = 0.758, n = 6) significantly altered the proportion of modulated units. (Figure S4B).
Finally, we performed two supplementary analyses to evaluate the robustness of our approach to detecting firing rate modulation: a sensitivity analysis assessing the proportion of modulated units at different firing rate thresholds for inclusion/exclusion and a data dropout analysis designed to control for the possibility that non-physiological stimulation artifacts may preclude the detection of temporally adjacent spiking. These results recapitulate our observation that units with higher baseline firing are most likely to exhibit modulation and suggest that suppression in firing rate is not solely attributable to amplifier saturation following stimulation (Figure S5).
Neurons Exhibit Heterogenous Responses to Theta Burst Stimulation
Recent studies have reported enhanced neural plasticity (via intracranial local field potential recordings and evoked responses) following repetitive direct electrical stimulation (Herrero et al., 2021; Huang et al., 2024, 2019; Keller et al., 2018). Accordingly, we hypothesized that recorded units would predominantly exhibit enhanced spiking in response to intracranial TBS of the BLA. Similarly, individual units exhibited highly variable responses to stimulation with respect to onset latency (rapid vs. delayed), duration (transient vs. durable), and valence (enhancement vs. suppression) (Figure 2B).
The most common epoch for firing rate modulation was during the 1 s epoch in which TBS was delivered (25.0% of all neurons). Smaller subsets were modulated only in the 1 s post-stimulation epoch (6.4%) or in both the during- and post-stimulation epochs (1.3%). A similar trend was observed for modulation in the no-stim condition: 10.9% during, 5.8% post, and 1.3% for both. Suppression was most common among modulated units during stimulation (56.4%), whereas enhancement was the dominant response post-stimulation (70.0%). In contrast, enhancement was most common within both epochs across no-stim trials (58.5% during, 66.7% post). The mean (± SD) absolute z-scored difference in firing rate across stimulation trials (relative to pre-trial ISI) was z = 0.60 (± 0.58) and z = 0.43 (± 0.27) for the during- and post-stimulation epochs, respectively. Across no-stim trials, we observed a mean absolute z-scored difference of z = 0.38 (± 0.24) and z = 0.30 (± 0.18) in analogous epochs (Figure 3E).
Association Between Neuronal Modulation and Memory Performance is Unclear
Next, we investigated the link between stimulation and performance during the visual recognition memory task. To this end, we first used a linear mixed-effects model to examine the effect of condition (stim, no-stim) on memory performance (d’) across trials in each session, with individual sessions treated as a random effect (intercept). Experiment type was also included as a fixed effect since data were aggregated across four highly similar experiments with minor differences in the content of visual stimuli, number of trials, stimulation parameters, and testing intervals. We did not observe an overall effect of memory enhancement (p > 0.05) when controlling for subject-level variability (Figure 4A). The lack of a memory enhancement effect may be associated with high hit rates limiting sensitivity (mean ± SD) (75.7% ± 13.5% for no-stim trials, 75.0% ± 14.3% for stim trials), and considerable variability among false alarm rates across participants (17.9% ± 17.4%, range 0–70%; Figure 4B). At the level of individual sessions, we observed enhanced memory (Δd’ > +0.2) in 43.3%, impaired memory (Δd’ < -0.2) in 36.7%, and negligible change (-0.2 ≤ Δd’ ≤ 0.2) in 20.0% when comparing performance between the stim and no-stim conditions; a threshold of Δd’ ± 0.2 was chosen for this classification based on the defined range of a “small effect” for Cohen’s d.
Discussion
Theta-burst stimulation is an efficient and validated paradigm for inducing long-term potentiation (LTP) in neural circuits (Larson and Munkácsy, 2015). Additionally, intracranial TBS was recently shown to promote region-specific short-term plasticity (Herrero et al., 2021; Huang et al., 2024) and entrain frequency-matched oscillations (Solomon et al., 2021). At present, however, there is an incomplete understanding of how these population-level responses to stimulation relate to a modulation in the activity of individual neurons, which are thought to be the substrate of memory encoding and retrieval (Hebb, 1949). Here, we address this knowledge gap by characterizing neuronal firing recorded from microelectrodes in humans undergoing intracranial TBS of the BLA. Our experimental design focused explicitly on stimulation of the BLA, given our prior work that seeks to understand amygdala-mediated memory enhancement in humans (Inman et al., 2023, 2020, 2018).
We observed neurons distributed throughout the hippocampus, orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and amygdala responsive to direct electrical TBS. The effect of TBS on firing rate was heterogeneous with respect to onset, duration, and valence. Previous work characterizing local-field potential responses to intracranial TBS observed similarly bidirectional effects throughout the brain suggestive of short-term plasticity (Huang et al., 2024). Few studies, however, have characterized the impact of exogenous stimulation on the spiking activity of individual neurons. One study reported a long-lasting reduction in neural excitability among parietal neurons, with variable onset time and recovery following continuous transcranial TBS in non-human primates (Romero et al., 2022). Other emerging evidence suggests that transcranial direct current stimulation may entrain neuronal spiking (Krause et al., 2019) and that stimulation-evoked modulation of spiking may meaningfully impact behavioral performance on cognitive tasks (Fehring et al., 2024). An alternative approach has focused on the delivery of spatially selective microstimulation resembling the extracellular currents that normally modulate neuronal activity—this methodology has been used to bi-directionally drive neuronal firing in human temporal cortex (Youssef et al., 2023) and enhance memory specificity for images following stimulation (Titiz et al., 2017).
Although subsets of neurons from each region we sampled were responsive to stimulation, we observed the greatest difference in the proportion of modulated units across conditions in the hippocampus, orbitofrontal cortex, and amygdala. This regional selectivity is to be expected, given that numerous studies have characterized how structural, functional, and effective connectivity among brain regions predicts the effects of stimulation (Fox et al., 2020; Huang et al., 2024, 2019; Keller et al., 2018, 2011; Solomon et al., 2018; Stiso et al., 2019). We also observed that units with greater baseline activity were most likely to exhibit modulated firing rates following stimulation. Other studies have identified firing patterns and waveform properties that differ between inhibitory and excitatory neurons in humans (Barthó et al., 2004; Keller et al., 2010; Peyrache et al., 2012; Quyen et al., 2008). For example, baseline firing rate disambiguates “regular-spiking” and “fast-spiking” neurons, which are presumed to represent pyramidal cells and interneurons, respectively. This may suggest that inhibitory interneurons are especially sensitive to TBS. However, further analysis of waveform properties (e.g., valley-to-peak height, half-peak width) is needed to more reliably classify neuronal cell types. Future research that seeks to identify specific characteristics of human neurons that predict responses to stimulation would be informative, given recent reports that the extent to which electrical fields entrain neuronal spiking may be specific to distinct classes of cells (Lee et al., 2024).
Modulation in neuronal activity was defined by contrasting firing rates before, during, and after TBS across trials. In doing so, we were able to characterize coarse differences in activity indicative of enhancement or suppression. This approach, however, did not allow for analysis of more subtle, nuanced effects such as entrainment of spiking to individual bursts or pulses of TBS. Characterizations of rhythmicity in firing were challenging, given that most of the neurons we identified exhibit sparse activity with low baseline firing rates, and stimulation often resulted in further suppression of spiking.
Although stimulation artifacts resulted in amplitude threshold crossings that may be spuriously interpreted as a neuronal spike, we implemented several methods to mitigate the influence of non-physiological activity. First, the characteristics of each unit (e.g., waveform shape) were manually inspected during spike sorting and further quantified using several quality control metrics (e.g., interspike intervals); stimulation resulted in a stereotyped response that was easily detectable and removed from subsequent analyses. Additionally, we tested for modulation both during stimulation and in the post-stimulation epochs—a period in which no artifact was present. Contrary to what would be expected if stimulation artifacts were falsely elevating firing rates, we observed predominantly suppression during stimulation and enhancement post-stimulation.
Since we collected our microelectrode recordings in the context of a visual recognition memory task, we tested whether stimulation resulted in a change in memory performance. Although we hypothesized that stimulation would improve memory performance, we did not identify an apparent stimulation-related memory enhancement when controlling for individual differences, in contrast to our prior work (Inman et al., 2018). The absence of such an effect may be, at least in part, attributable to the considerable variability that we observed in this cohort; indeed, baseline memory performance and individual differences have been shown to account for a substantial portion of the variability in this amygdala-mediated memory enhancement effect (Hollearn et al., 2024).
Several studies on rats have demonstrated that brief electrical stimulation of the BLA can prioritize the consolidation of specific memories (Bass et al., 2014, 2012; Bass and Manns, 2015; Manns and Bass, 2016). These pro-memory effects emerged ∼24 hours post-encoding and appear to be hippocampal-dependent (Bass et al., 2014), despite not resulting in a net change in the firing rates of hippocampal pyramidal neurons; instead, BLA stimulation resulted in brief periods of spike-field and field-field synchrony within CA3–CA1 in the low-gamma frequency range (30–55 Hz), which may facilitate spike-timing-dependent plasticity in recently active neurons (Bass and Manns, 2015).
The present study did not investigate interactions between spiking activity and local field potentials. How exactly the activity of single neurons is aggregated to produce local field potentials, which in turn interact with neuronal ensembles distributed throughout the brain, remains an active area of research (Herreras, 2016; Kajikawa and Schroeder, 2011; Manning et al., 2009; Teleńczuk et al., 2020). One recent study that leveraged closed-loop stimulation targeting memory consolidation during sleep observed neuronal spiking with greater phase-locking to medial temporal lobe slow-wave activity following stimulation (Geva-Sagiv et al., 2023); neuronal phase-locking, particularly to hippocampal theta oscillations, has long been associated with robust memory encoding and retrieval (Jacobs et al., 2007; Rutishauser et al., 2010; Schonhaut et al., 2024; Yoo et al., 2021). Further characterization of these spike-field interactions and refinement of closed-loop stimulation methods may provide a means for precisely modulating neuronal dynamics, for example, by entraining neuronal spiking that is phase-aligned to endogenous hippocampal theta oscillations to selectively enhance the encoding or retrieval of memories (Hasselmo, 2005; Hasselmo et al., 2002; Hasselmo and Stern, 2014; Siegle and Wilson, 2014).
Finally, we performed an exploratory analysis of neuronal pseudo-population activity, which suggests that hippocampal neurons exhibit robust changes in firing rate coactivity in response to BLA stimulation. Related research has similarly described how BLA stimulation can induce synchronous firing of hippocampal neurons, which has memory-enhancing effects (Bass and Manns, 2015). Other studies have leveraged similar, low-dimensional representations of population dynamics to describe how coordinated neural activity facilitates inferential reasoning and memory retrieval within the medial frontal cortex and hippocampus, respectively (Courellis et al., 2024; Minxha et al., 2020). Thus, a greater understanding of how neuronal coactivity may be precisely modulated by stimulation may help to refine therapeutic interventions targeting complex cognition and computation.
Conclusions
By characterizing patterns of neuronal modulation evoked by intracranial TBS, we provide new insights that link micro- and macroscale responses to stimulation of the human brain. These insights advance our limited understanding of how focal electrical fields influence neuronal firing at the single-cell level and motivate future neuromodulatory therapies that aim to recapitulate specific patterns of activity implicated in cognition and memory.
Resource availability
Custom Python analysis scripts used in the manuscript are publicly available on GitHub (https://github.com/Justin-Campbell/BLAESUnits). Deidentified neural recordings may be made available upon reasonable request.
Supplemental figures

Unit quality metrics.
(A) Number of units detected per implanted microelectrode bundle. (B) Mean firing rate (Hz) across recording session. (C) Percent of interspike intervals < 3 ms. (D) Interspike interval coefficient of variation. (E) Mean presence ratio of firing within units (1 s bins). (F) Signal-to-noise ratio of unit waveform peak. (G) Mean signal-to-noise ratio across the entire unit waveform. (H) Representative example of stereotyped, high-amplitude stimulation-artifact waveform; non-physiological waveforms were excluded from analysis.

Characterization of units based on laterality relative to stimulation.
(A) Unit counts on the contralateral (Contra) or ipsilateral (Ipsi) side of stimulation. (B) Unit counts separated by laterality and region. (C) Stacked histograms of Euclidean distance between microelectrode bundle location and stimulation contacts, separated by laterality (bin size = 5 mm).

Analysis of pseudo-population activity within regions, separated by laterality relative to stimulation.
Pseudo-population activity was characterized within each region via a linear dimensionality reduction on the trial-averaged firing rates. The temporal dynamics of each region’s first three principal components are shown for (A) units ipsilateral to stimulation and (B) units contralateral to stimulation. HIP = hippocampus (coral), OFC = orbitofrontal cortex (yellow), AMY = amygdala (blue), ACC = anterior cingulate cortex (purple).

Sub-analysis of stimulation parameters used across experiments.
Comparison of the proportion of stimulation-modulated units across sessions with 1 mA (n = 23) vs. 0.5 mA (n = 7). (B) Comparison of the proportion of stimulation-modulated units across sessions testing distinct pulse frequencies: 33 Hz vs. 80 Hz (n = 1) and 50 Hz vs. 80 Hz (n = 6). The values above individual bars represent the number of sessions using that stimulation parameter. NS = not significant.

Control analyses for the detection of modulated units.
The same permutation-based analyses reported in the manuscript were repeated under different control conditions. The percent of units (total n = 203) that met the firing rate threshold for inclusion (pink), the percent of included units modulated in the stim condition (purple), and the percent of included units modulated in the no-stim condition are shown. (A) The threshold for inclusion of units was varied from 0–3 Hz (0.1 step size); the black dashed line represents the ≥ 0.1 Hz threshold used in the manuscript. (B) To control for the possibility that non-physiological stimulation artifacts may preclude the detection of temporally adjacent spiking, we removed segments of data beginning at the onset of each burst of pulses (0–60 ms, 5 ms step size). Identical temporal windows were removed from the corresponding pre- and post-stimulation epochs to mitigate effects resulting solely from summation over different epoch sizes (reduced spike counts with shorter windows).
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to all the patients who participated in the study.
Additional information
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (T32NS115723; K23NS114178), the National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH120194), the National Science Foundation (NSF2124252, NSF1747505), and the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation (2023 NARSAD Young Investigator Grant). This study was conducted as part of a National Institutes of Health clinical trial (NCT05065450).
CRediT Author Statement
Justin Campbell: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Data Curation, Writing – Original Draft, Writing – Review & Editing, Visualization; Rhiannon Cowan: Conceptualization, Data Curation, Writing – Review & Editing; Krista Wahlstrom: Investigation, Writing – Review & Editing, Project Administration; Martina Hollearn: Writing – Review & Editing; Dylan Jensen: Writing – Review & Editing; Tyler Davis: Software, Writing – Review & Editing; Shervin Rahimpour: Resources, Writing – Review & Editing; Ben Shofty: Resources, Writing – Review & Editing; Amir Arain: Resources, Writing – Review & Editing; John Rolston: Resources, Writing – Review & Editing; Stephan Hamann: Writing – Review & Editing; Shuo Wang: Resources, Writing – Review & Editing; Lawrence Eisenman: Resources, Writing – Review & Editing; James Swift: Software, Investigation, Writing – Review & Editing; Tao Xie: Investigation, Writing – Review & Editing; Peter Brunner: Software, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – Review & Editing; Joe Manns: Conceptualization, Supervision, Project Administration, Writing – Review & Editing; Cory Inman: Supervision, Resources, Project Administration, Funding Acquisition, Writing – Review & Editing; Elliot Smith: Conceptualization, Supervision, Resources, Writing – Review & Editing; Jon Willie: Investigation, Supervision, Resources, Project Administration, Funding Acquisition, Writing – Review & Editing.
References
- Characterization of Neocortical Principal Cells and Interneurons by Network Interactions and Extracellular FeaturesJ Neurophysiol 92:600–608https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01170.2003
- Memory-Enhancing Amygdala Stimulation Elicits Gamma Synchrony in the HippocampusBehav Neurosci 129:244–256https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000052
- Amygdala-mediated enhancement of memory for specific events depends on the hippocampusNeurobiol Learn Mem 107:37–41https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.10.020
- Event-Specific Enhancement of Memory via Brief Electrical Stimulation to the Basolateral Complex of the Amygdala in RatsBehav Neurosci 126:204–208https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026462
- Abstract representations emerge in human hippocampal neurons during inferenceNature :1–9https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-024-07799-x
- LeGUI: A Fast and Accurate Graphical User Interface for Automated Detection and Anatomical Localization of Intracranial ElectrodesFront Neurosci 15:769872https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2021.769872
- Interaction between the Amygdala and the Medial Temporal Lobe Memory System Predicts Better Memory for Emotional EventsNeuron 42:855–863https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00289-2
- Direct current stimulation modulates prefrontal cell activity and behaviour without inducing seizure-like firingBrain awae 273https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awae273
- Intrinsic network architecture predicts the effects elicited by intracranial electrical stimulation of the human brainNat Hum Behav 4:1039–1052https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0910-1
- Augmenting hippocampal–prefrontal neuronal synchrony during sleep enhances memory consolidation in humansNat Neurosci :1–11https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-023-01324-5
- Oscillations and Episodic Memory: Addressing the Synchronization/Desynchronization ConundrumTrends Neurosci 39:16–25https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2015.11.004
- What is the function of hippocampal theta rhythm?—Linking behavioral data to phasic properties of field potential and unit recording data. Hippocampus 15:936–949https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20116
- A Proposed Function for Hippocampal Theta Rhythm: Separate Phases of Encoding and Retrieval Enhance Reversal of Prior LearningNeural Comput 14:793–817https://doi.org/10.1162/089976602317318965
- Theta rhythm and the encoding and retrieval of space and timeNeuroimage 85:656–666https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.022
- The organization of behavior; A neuropsychological theory. New York: John Wiley and SonsInc https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.37303405110
- How the amygdala affects emotional memory by altering brain network propertiesNeurobiol Learn Mem 112:2–16https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2014.02.005
- Local Field Potentials: Myths and MisunderstandingsFront Neural Circuits 10:101https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2016.00101
- Inducing neuroplasticity through intracranial θ-burst stimulation in the human sensorimotor cortexJ Neurophysiol 126:1723–1739https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00320.2021
- Exploring individual differences in amygdala-mediated memory modulation. Cogn, AffectBehav Neurosci :1–22https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-024-01250-4
- Intracortical Dynamics Underlying Repetitive Stimulation Predicts Changes in Network ConnectivityJ Neurosci 39:6122–6135https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0535-19.2019
- Theta-burst direct electrical stimulation remodels human brain networksNat Commun 15:6982https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-51443-1
- Human amygdala stimulation effects on emotion physiology and emotional experienceNeuropsychologia 145:106722https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.019
- Discovering how the amygdala shapes human behavior: From lesion studies to neuromodulationNeuron https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2023.09.040
- Direct electrical stimulation of the amygdala enhances declarative memory in humansProc National Acad Sci 115:98–103https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1714058114
- Brain Oscillations Control Timing of Single-Neuron Activity in HumansJ Neurosci 27:3839–3844https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4636-06.2007
- How Local Is the Local Field Potential?Neuron 72:847–858https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.029
- Intrinsic functional architecture predicts electrically evoked responses in the human brainProc Natl Acad Sci 108:10308–10313https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1019750108
- Induction and quantification of excitability changes in human cortical networksJ Neurosci 38:1088–17https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1088-17.2018
- Heterogeneous neuronal firing patterns during interictal epileptiform discharges in the human cortexBrain 133:1668–1681https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awq112
- The structural and functional connectivity of the amygdala: From normal emotion to pathological anxietyBehav Brain Res 223:403–410https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.04.025
- Transcranial alternating current stimulation entrains single-neuron activity in the primate brainProc Natl Acad Sci 116:5747–5755https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1815958116
- Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memoryNat Rev Neurosci 7:54–64https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1825
- Theta-burst LTPBrain Res 1621:38–50https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.10.034
- Cell-class-specific electric field entrainment of neural activityNeuron https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2024.05.009
- Broadband Shifts in Local Field Potential Power Spectra Are Correlated with Single-Neuron Spiking in HumansJ Neurosci 29:13613–13620https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2041-09.2009
- The Amygdala and Prioritization of Declarative MemoriesCurr Dir Psychol Sci 25:261–265https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416654456
- Functional neuroanatomy of amygdalohippocampal interconnections and their role in learning and memoryJ Neurosci Res 95:797–820https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.23709
- Making lasting memories: Remembering the significantProc Natl Acad Sci 110:10402–10407https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1301209110
- The Amygdala Modulates The Consolidation of Memories of Emotionally Arousing ExperiencesAnnu Rev Neurosci 27:1–28https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144157
- Amygdala Modulation of Memory Consolidation: Interaction with Other Brain SystemsNeurobiol Learn Mem 78:539–552https://doi.org/10.1006/nlme.2002.4082
- Advances in human intracranial electroencephalography research, guidelines and good practicesNeuroimage 260:119438https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119438
- Flexible recruitment of memory-based choice representations by the human medial frontal cortexScience 368:eaba3313https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aba3313
- Scikit-learn: Machine Learning in PythonArxiv
- Spatiotemporal dynamics of neocortical excitation and inhibition during human sleepProc Natl Acad Sci 109:1731–1736https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1109895109
- Emotion and Cognition: Insights from Studies of the Human AmygdalaAnnu Rev Psychol 57:27–53https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070234
- Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complexCurr Opin Neurobiol 14:198–202https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.015
- Contributions of the Amygdala to Emotion Processing: From Animal Models to Human BehaviorNeuron 48:175–187https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025
- Neuronal activity in the human amygdala and hippocampus enhances emotional memory encodingNat Hum Behav 7:754–764https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01502-8
- Cell Type- Specific Firing during Ripple Oscillations in the Hippocampal Formation of HumansJ Neurosci 28:6104–6110https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0437-08.2008
- Encoding of emotional memories depends on amygdala and hippocampus and their interactionsNat Neurosci 7:278–285https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1190
- Amygdala-hippocampal interactions in synaptic plasticity and memory formationNeurobiol Learn Mem 184:107490https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2021.107490
- Neural effects of continuous theta- burst stimulation in macaque parietal neuronseLife 11:e65536https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.65536
- Stress, memory and the amygdalaNat Rev Neurosci 10:423–433https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2651
- Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fMRINeuroImage 45:614–626https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.030
- Human memory strength is predicted by theta-frequency phase-locking of single neuronsNature 464:903–907https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08860
- MTL neurons phase-lock to human hippocampal thetaeLife 13:e85753https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.85753
- Mineralocorticoid Receptor Blockade Prevents Stress-Induced Modulation of Multiple Memory Systems in the Human BrainBiol Psychiatry 74:801–808https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.06.001
- Statsmodels: Econometric and Statistical Modeling with PythonProc 9th Python Sci Conf :92–96https://doi.org/10.25080/majora-92bf1922-011
- Robust, automatic spike sorting using mixtures of multivariate t-distributionsJ Neurosci Methods 127:111–122https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0270(03)00120-1
- Enhancement of encoding and retrieval functions through theta phase-specific manipulation of hippocampuseLife 3:e03061https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.03061
- Medial temporal lobe functional connectivity predicts stimulation-induced theta powerNat Commun 9:4437https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-06876-w
- Theta-burst stimulation entrains frequency-specific oscillatory responsesBrain Stimul 14:1271–1284https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2021.08.014
- A validated network of effective amygdala connectivityNeuroImage 36:736–745https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.022
- White Matter Network Architecture Guides Direct Electrical Stimulation through Optimal State TransitionsCell Reports 28:2554–2566https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2019.08.008
- Ten Years of Theta Burst Stimulation in Humans: Established Knowledge, Unknowns and ProspectsBrain Stimul 9:323–335https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2016.01.006
- Modelling unitary fields and the single- neuron contribution to local field potentials in the hippocampusJ Physiol 598:3957–3972https://doi.org/10.1113/jp279452
- Theta-burst microstimulation in the human entorhinal area improves memory specificityeLife 6:e29515https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.29515
- SciPy 1.0: fundamental algorithms for scientific computing in PythonNat Methods 17:261–272https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-019-0686-2
- Neurons in the human medial temporal lobe track multiple temporal contexts during episodic memory processingNeuroImage 245:118689https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118689
- Neuronal Spiking Responses to Direct Electrical Microstimulation in the Human CortexJ Neurosci 43:4448–4460https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1666-22.2023
- Amygdala-hippocampal dynamics during salient information processingNat Commun 8:14413https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms14413
- Multiplexing of Theta and Alpha Rhythms in the Amygdala- Hippocampal Circuit Supports Pattern Separation of Emotional InformationNeuron 102:887–898https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.03.025
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2025, Campbell et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 93
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.