Abstract
Fear responses to perceived danger are critical for survival, as they prompt the individual to respond to threats and avoid harm. However, excessive fear can impede normal biological processes and become harmful. This study investigates the neural mechanisms underlying two distinct forms of fear—phasic and sustained—in male and female mice, with a focus on the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) and corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) signaling. Phasic fear is characterized by immediate responses to clear threats, while sustained fear is driven by ambiguous or uncertain cues and persists longer. Using rodent models, we found that sustained fear, modeled by partial fear conditioning, induced greater arousal and BNST activity in males, especially during ambiguous threat cues. In contrast, females exhibited reduced BNST and BNSTCRF activity, highlighting significant sex differences in fear learning and expression. Additionally, CRF is crucial for appropriate fear response in females, as CRF knockdown led to increased fear responses, but had no effect in males. These sex-specific differences could help inform the development of targeted treatments for anxiety and trauma-related disorders, which disproportionately affect women.
Introduction
Fear is a protective response to perceived danger that allows an organism to identify and respond to threats to avoid harm. While fear is essential for survival, excessive fear can interfere with crucial biological processes1–5, emphasizing the need for accurate risk assessment to maintain overall well-being. In preclinical research, risk assessment is often measured through cue-danger associations in fear conditioning paradigms, where a tone cue predicts an aversive event. The resulting fear response to the tone is used to assess fear learning. Fear conditioning paradigms can vary in how cue-danger associations are paired, leading to different behavioral states underpinned by distinct neural mechanisms. For instance, phasic fear is considered an adaptive response and is characterized by reaction to a clear and discrete cue that dissipates rapidly once the threat is no longer present. In rodents, this is often modeled using a fully reinforced cued-fear paradigm, where a tone exposure always co-terminates with a foot shock, thus becoming a discrete cue for this threat. Conversely, sustained fear is a heightened state of arousal and anxiety that is not clearly associated with specific cues and lasts for longer periods of time6,7. Rodent models for this include a partially reinforced cued-fear paradigm, where tones do not always co-terminate with shock and therefore become uncertain cues for danger8.
Most fear learning studies have been conducted in male rodents and reveal that the basolateral and central amygdala play key roles in mediating fear responses. These brain regions are activated during exposure to the tone and the subsequent fear reaction, and their inhibition disrupts normal fear processing. While the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), part of the extended amygdala, is not always included in canonical fear circuitry, its recruitment is particularly crucial when there is uncertainty regarding when or if harm will occur and is thought to drive sustained fear7–12. However, the precise role that the BNST plays in uncertain fear processing remains unclear, particularly in females. The corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) system in the BNST is also implicated in sustained fear. CRF infusions to the BNST enhance sustained fear and emotional memory recall13, while CRF receptor antagonism in the BNST blocks sustained fear7,14. Interestingly, overexpression of CRF in BNST-CRF-expressing (BNSTCRF) neurons prior to conditioning weakens sustained fear15. These findings suggest that BNSTCRF neurons and CRF inputs to the BNST are both important for fear expression, though the precise mechanisms by which they influence fear remain unclear.
Critically, though mood disorders are more prevalent in women and non-binary individuals, preclinical studies have classically only included males16,17. Sex differences in defensive behavioral strategies, BNST structure and function, and CRF signaling have been reported, and thus, investigation into how these features shape negative emotional states is prudent18–25. Studies suggest that females have more BNSTCRF neurons26,27 in this sexually dimorphic region, which may drive innate sex differences in fear learning and expression. In this study, we directly examine the contribution of BNSTCRF signaling to phasic and sustained fear in male and female mice to test the overarching hypothesis that plasticity in BNSTCRF neurons drive distinct behavioral responses to unpredictable threat in males and females. Since sustained fear paradigms better capture the symptoms of human anxiety, understanding the neural correlates that distinguish phasic and sustained fear is critical for improving treatments for anxiety disorders7.
Methods
Mice
All experiments were conducted in accordance with the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill’s Institutional Animal Care Use Committee’s guidelines. All mice were group housed and maintained on a 12h/12h light-dark cycle with lights on at 7 AM and had access to standard rodent chow and water ad libitum. Male and Female C57BL/6J mice >8 weeks of age were purchased from Jackson Laboratories (Bar Harbor, ME). CRF-cre, and CRFfl/flmice were bred in house and maintained on a C57BL/6J background.
Surgery
Mice were anesthetized using isoflurane (4% induction, 1-2% maintenance). Mice were administered buprenorphine (.1 mg/kg, s.c.) during surgery, and were also given acetaminophen (80 mg/200mL in drinking water) one day before and for at least 3 days following surgery. AAV constructs were delivered to target regions using Hamilton microsyringes at the following coordinates; dBNST: AP +.3mm, ML +/- .9mm, DV -4.35. vBNST: AP +.3mm, ML +/- .9mm, DV -4.6. For fiber photometry experiments, fiber optic cannulae (FOC) were cut to length and implanted at the same coordinates and cemented to the skull with dental cement (C&B Metabond, Parkell). For miniature microscope experiments, GRIN lenses (.6 x 7.3mm) with integrated baseplates (Inscopix, Mountain View, CA) were implanted .2mm above the target coordinates and lowered at a rate of .2mm/min. Mice recovered for >3 weeks prior to the start of experiments.
Viruses
For CRF deletion experiments, AAV5-CaMKiia-cre-GFP and AAV5-CaMKiia- GFP were obtained from the UNC Vector Core (Chapel Hill, NC). Viruses for fiber photometry and 1-photon calcium imaging detailed below.
Behavior
Fear conditioning
Fear conditioning was conducted using a three-day protocol. On the first day (habituation), mice were placed into a fear conditioning chamber (context A, Med Associates, St. Albans, VT) equipped with a shock grid floor and clear plexiglass walls, and allowed to forage for five minutes. Context A was cleaned with a 20% ethanol + 1% vanilla extract solution to provide a scent cue. On day 2 (fear learning), mice were returned to context A. Following a 3-minute baseline, mice were presented with tone- shock pairings (tone: 30 s, 80 dB, 3 KHz, shock: 0.6 mA, 2 s), or unpaired tones depending on the experimental group. Mice subjected to fully-reinforced fear (Full) received 3 tone-shock pairings separated by inter-trial intervals of 60 and 90s. Mice undergoing partially reinforced fear (Part) received 3 tone-shock pairings as well as 3 unpaired tones interspersed such that tones 1, 2, and 4 ended in shock, and 3, 5, and 6 did not. Inter-trial intervals were as follows: 60-20-40-20-20. Control mice (Ctrl) received 6 tones at the same timing as the Part group, except the mice did not receive foot shocks. Following tone-shock presentations, mice remained in context A for a 2-minute consolidation period (End). On the third day, mice were placed into a novel context with white plastic flooring, curved walls, and cleaned with 0.5% acetic acid. Following a 3- minute baseline, 6 tones (identical to fear learning) were presented separated by a random ITI of 20-60 s. Behavior hardware was controlled by Ethovision XT software (Noldus Inc.).
Novelty-Induced suppression of feeding (NSF)
Mice were food deprived overnight prior to the start of NSF. On test day, a single pellet of standard chow was placed into the center of a brightly lit (1350 lux) white Plexiglas open field chamber. The floor was covered with normal cage bedding. Mice were placed into the corner of the chamber. The trial was ended when the mouse ate for a period of 3 seconds or more. If mice did not eat, the trial was ended after 10 minutes and mice were assigned a maximum latency of 600 seconds. After the test, mice were returned to a dimly lit clean cage and allowed to eat for 10 minutes, and amount consumed was recorded.
Acoustic startle
Mice were semi-restrained in Plexiglas tubes mounted to a platform with an accelerometer attached and placed inside a sound attenuating chamber (all hardware and software from San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA). This context was separate from fear conditioning chambers. Following a 5-minute acclimation period, white noise bursts of varying intensity were played, and startle responses were measured. Four different stimulus intensities were tested: 90 dB, 105 dB, 120dB, and 0 dB (control) separated by a random ITI of 30-60 seconds. Each session consisted of 10 blocks of 4 trials, one of each intensity, such that there were 10 total trials at each stimulus intensity. Startle response peaks were detected automatically using Startle Response Lab software.
Fiber Photometry
GCaMP6s (AAV5-hsyn-GCaMP6s, UPenn Vector Core, Philadelphia, PA) was injected into the BNST of C57BL/6J mice and fibers were implanted unilaterally in the BNST. Fiber optic cannulae (200μm, .37nA, Neurophotometrics, San Diego, CA) were cut to length and tested for light transmission prior to implantation. Prior to behavioral testing, mice were habituated to patch cord tethering daily for 3 days. Using a Neurophotometrics FP3001 system (Neurophotometrics, San Diego, CA), alternating pulses of 470 nm and 415 nm LED light (∼50 µW, 40 Hz) were bandpass filtered and focused onto a branching patch cord (Doric, Quebec City, Quebec) by a 20x objective lens. A custom-built Arduino was used to time lock recordings to behavior. Custom MATLAB scripts were used for all analysis. Background fluorescence was subtracted from 415 and 470 traces, then traces were lowpass filtered at 5 Hz and fit to a biexponential curve. Curve fit was subtracted from each trace to correct for baseline drift. dF/F for 415 and 470 traces were calculated (raw signal-fitted signal)/(fitted signal), and traces were z-scored. The 415 signal was fit to the 470 signal using non-negative robust linear regression to correct for motion artifacts 28. Event analysis: Events were identified from processed traces using a custom MATLAB script, and were defined as peaks greater than 2* the mean absolute deviation of baseline (first minute of each recording). Peak timing was then aligned with freezing data to determine whether each event occurred during freezing or mobility.
1- Photon Calcium Imaging
Prior to the start of behavioral testing, mice were habituated to miniature head- mounted microendoscope attachment daily for 3 days. For all experiments, 470 LED light was delivered at .9 - 1.1mW, and data were recorded at 20 Hz using an Inscopix nVoke imaging system (Inscopix, Mountain View, CA). Recordings were time-locked to behavior hardware via TTL. Data were analyzed using Inscopix Data Processing Software. Briefly, data were spatially downsampled by a factor of 4, and temporally downsampled by a factor of 2 to reduce computational load. Videos were then spatial bandpass filtered, motion corrected, and normalized with dF/F to prepare the video for automated cell detection using PCA-ICA. Detected regions of interest were then manually accepted or rejected based on signal quality, and overlapping regions were discarded. Events were then identified as events with a minimum peak of 3* the median absolute deviation and minimum decay time of .2 s. Traces were then further processed using a custom MATLAB script. Briefly, traces were z-scored, and peri-event plots were generated. Responses to tone and shock were determined for each trial for each cell using a Wilcoxon rank-sum test comparing the 2-second window before and after stimulus. For GCaMP recordings in BNSTCRF, GCaMP8m AAV1-syn-flex-GCaMP8m (Addgene) was diluted 1:1 in sterile PBS and injected unilaterally into the BNST.
Automated Behavior Tracking
Behavior videos were tracked using DeepLabCut software that uses deep neural networks to estimate mouse position 29. Behavior was then scored by analyzing DLC tracking output using SimBA 30, a machine learning platform that can be trained to identify behaviors. A classifier for freezing behavior was created in house. A custom MATLAB script (Mathworks, Natick, Massachusetts) was used to determine freezing bout start and end times, and to calculate % freezing during various behavior epochs.
Histology
To verify virus, fiber, and GRIN lens placement, mice were transcardially perfused with 30mls each of phosphate buffered saline and 4% paraformaldehyde. Brains were post-fixed in 4% PFA for 24h after extraction, then sectioned on a vibratome. GCaMP fluorescence was amplified using immunohistochemistry. Mice with off-target virus expression, fiber, or lens placement were excluded from analysis.
Results
Partially Reinforced Fear Conditioning Drives Hyperarousal in Males
To characterize the behavioral profiles and underlying neural processing that distinguish phasic and sustained fear, we subjected male and female C57BL/6J mice to either partially reinforced fear conditioning (Part), fully reinforced fear conditioning (Full), or tone only exposure (Ctrl). To model phasic fear, we used fully reinforced fear paradigm, where mice are exposed to 3 tone-shock pairings such that 100% of tones are paired with a shock. To model sustained fear, we used a partially reinforced fear paradigm where mice are presented with 6 tones, where only the first, second, and fourth tones are paired with a shock8. Twenty-four hours after fear conditioning, all mice were subjected to a fear recall protocol consisting of 6 tone presentations (Fig 1A). Freezing was scored as a measure of defensive behavior.

Partially reinforced fear drives a hyperarousal phenotype in males.
A, Schematic of behavior chambers and tone/shock presentations for fully- and partially reinforced fear conditioning and recall. B, Freezing during baseline and 0-28 seconds of tone presentations in Ctrl (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(3.137, 69.01)=1.449 ns, sex: F(1, 22)=1.717 ns, tone x sex: F(6, 132)=0.2104 ns), Full (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(2.046, 45.01)=45.19 p<.0001, sex: F(1,22)=0.4527 ns, tone x sex: F(3,66)=1.282 ns), and Part (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(3.218, 70.79)=70.73 p<.0001, sex: F(1,22)=0.3841 ns, tone x sex: F(6, 132)=0.4960 ns) fear. C, Freezing for all tones from 0-28 seconds during fear conditioning (Two- way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(2, 66)=53.06 p<.0001, sex: F(1,66)=0.4488 ns, fear x sex: F(2, 66)=0.4588 ns). D, Full tone 3 and Part tone 4 freezing during fear conditioning (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1, 44)=0.1043 ns, sex: F(1,44)=0.08787 ns, fear x sex: F(1, 44)=0.3009 ns). E, Part fear tone 3, 4, 5 freezing during conditioning (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(2, 44)=6.578 p<.01, sex: F(1, 22)=0.5318 ns, tone x sex: F(2, 44)=0.3213 ns), F, Freezing during recall baseline and 0-28 seconds of tone presentations in Ctrl (two- way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(3.394, 74.66)=1.449 ns, sex: F(1, 22)=1.261 ns, tone x sex: F(6, 132)=0.6956 ns), Full (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(4.980, 109.6)=12.19 p<.0001, sex: F(1,22)=0.1797 ns, tone x sex: F(6, 132)=1.274 ns), and Part (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(4.754, 104.6)=14.43 p<.0001, sex: F(1,22)=0.0483 ns, tone x sex: F(6, 132)=1.065 ns) fear. G, Freezing during baseline period in recall Full and Part fear (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1, 43)=2.732 ns, sex: F(1,43)=1.876 ns, fear x sex: F(1, 43)=1.776 ns). 1 male Part fear mice excluded as outlier. H, Freezing during recall first tone exposure (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1, 43)=1.228 ns, sex: F(1,43)=0.0021 ns, fear x sex: F(1, 43)=1.687 ns) 1 female Part fear mouse excluded as outlier. I, Average freezing for all tone presentations in Recall Full and Part Fear (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1, 43)=7.401 p<.01, sex: F(1,43)=0.7413 ns, fear x sex: F(1, 43)=0.5288 ns) 1 female Part fear mouse excluded as outlier. J, Behavioral timeline for Novelty- induced-Suppression-of-Feeding (NSF) anxiety-like and Acoustic Startle arousal behavioral tests after fear conditioning. K, Freezing for all tones from 0-28 seconds during fear conditioning (Two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(2, 54)=57.10 p<.0001, sex: F(1,54)=0.7934 ns, fear x sex: F(2, 54)=0.3522 ns). L, Full tone 3 and Part tone 4 freezing during fear conditioning (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1, 36)=2.236 ns, sex: F(1,36)=0.5574 ns, fear x sex: F(1, 36)=0.3593 ns). M, Part fear tone 3, 4, 5 freezing during conditioning (two- way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(2, 36)=14.53 p<.0001, sex: F(1, 18)=0.2412 ns, tone x sex: F(2, 36)=1.541 ns), N, Latency to feed in the NSF. O, Food consumed during NSF refeed test. P, Acoustic startle response in males (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of fear (F(2,20)=11.61 p=.0005, main effect of stim intensity: F(1.808, 36.15)=166.9 p<.0001, interaction: F(4,40)=6.096 p=.0006, Tukey post-hoc: 120 dB Full v. Part p=.0395). 1 Ctrl 90dB male excluded as outlier. Q, Acoustic startle response in females (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of fear (F(2,33)=1.869 ns, main effect of stim intensity F(1.7452,57.57)=223.1 p<.0001). n=8-12/group. R, 120dB acoustic startle response in males and females (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of fear F(2,53)=10.01 p<.001, main effect of sex F(1,53)=9.951 p<.01, fear x sex: F(2, 53)=4.623 p<.05: Tukey post-hoc Males Ctrl vs. Part p<.0001, Males Full vs. Part p<.01, Part Males vs. Females p<.0001) 1 Male 90dB Ctrl mice excluded as outlier. n=8-12/group. All data are shown as mean ± SEM. * = p<.05, ** = p<.01, *** = p<.001, **** = p<.0001.
As expected, the tone-only control mice displayed minimal freezing behavior during conditioning, and, while freezing increased over time, there was no significant differences between male and female mice during either Full or Part fear acquisition (Fig. 1B). When comparing freezing to all tones, we find that freezing is significantly different between Ctrl, Full, and Part fear mice, but not between males and females (Fig 1C). Because freezing increases over time, it is possible that these differences are driven by differences in paradigm length. Therefore, we compared freezing to the last tone shock pairing (tone 3 in Full fear and tone 4 in Part fear) and found no differences between Full and Part fear mice (Fig 1D). This suggest that freezing differences between Full and Part fear conditioning are a product of Part fear consisting of more tones.
The Part fear paradigm allows for careful dissection of uncertain fear learning by allowing for within-mouse comparisons before and after uncertainty are introduced. As mice are shocked after tones 1 and 2, the third tone serves as a danger signal. However, as tone 3 does not end in shock, the fourth tone becomes an uncertain predictor of danger. Tone 4 again co-terminates with a shock, introducing further uncertainty. To understand how ambiguous threat impacts behavior, we compared freezing during tones 3, 4, and 5 in the Part fear group. Interestingly, regardless of sex, mice froze more during the 5th tone (Fig 1E). This is unlikely due to a gradual increase in freezing over time, as tone 3 and 4 response are indistinguishable.
During recall, Ctrl mice froze very little, and there were no differences in freezing between sexes for Ctrl, Full or Part Fear (Fig 1F). There were also no differences in freezing between Full and Part mice during the baseline period (Fig 1G), however, similarly to what Glover et al8 found, Part fear froze slightly less than Full fear mice during tone presentations, regardless of sex (Fig 1I).
Studies in rodents and humans have shown that paradigms in which aversive stimuli are uncertain or temporally unpredictable drive increased arousal and avoidance behaviors8,10,11,31,32. We subjected Ctrl, Full and Part fear mice to conditioning as described above followed by a novelty-induced suppression of feeding (NSF) test twenty-four hours after and an acoustic startle reflex test 48 hours after to assess how fear drives elements of avoidance behaviors and arousal respectively. Again, we found that during conditioning, Part fear mice froze more in the final tone than preceding tones (Fig 1K), but differences in freezing between Part and Full fear were not observed (Fig 1L). There were no differences between fear conditions or sexes in latency to feed (Fig 1N) or amount consumed during the refeed portion (Fig 1O) of the NSF test. In the acoustic startle test, males exhibited potentiated startle response after both Full and Part fear conditioning (Fig 1P) while females did not (fig 1Q). Notably, at the highest sound intensity, Part fear males showed a larger startle response than Full fear males (Fig 1R), suggesting that Part fear conditioning drives hyperarousal in males.
BNST dynamics differ during Part and Full fear
To characterize BNST dynamics, we injected a viral construct containing the genetically encoded calcium sensor GCaMP6s into the BNST of C57BL/6J mice, implanted fiber optic cannulae for fiber photometry, and recorded calcium activity during Full and Part Fear conditioning and recall (Fig 2A). Given that the BNST is recruited in unpredictable fear paradigms7,8,10,11, we hypothesized that the BNST would be recruited during both Full and Part fear conditioning, but show more activity in Part fear.

BNST is dynamically engaged during fear conditioning and recall.
A, Surgical schematic for fiber photometry experiments. B,C Schematic of tone and shock presentations during fear conditioning and recall. D, BNST representative images showing fiber placement. E, Fear Conditioning traces in males. F, BNST response to tone onset (0-2seconds) for all tones (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,30)=5.834 p<.05, sex: F(1, 30)=10.56 p<.01, fear x sex: F(1, 30)=0.5614 ns; Bonferroni’s post-hoc test: Part Males vs Females p=.0105). G, Comparison of Full tone 3 and Part tone 4 BNST response (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,30)=0.1296 ns, sex: F(1, 30)=4.216 p<.05, fear x sex: F(1, 30)=0.8026 ns). H, Fear Conditioning traces in females. I, Shock response in Full and Part fear conditioning averaged from t=28-30 across all shock trials (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,30)=0..7701 ns, sex: F(1, 30)=0.4407 ns, fear x sex: F(1, 30)=14.11 p<0.001, Bonferroni’s post- hoc: Males Full vs Part p=.0068, Full Males vs Females p=.0121). J, Final shock response in Full and Part fear conditioning averaged from t=28-30 (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,30)=3.142 ns p=.09, sex: F(1, 30)=0.2762 ns, fear x sex: F(1, 30)=5.682 p<0.05, Bonferroni’s post-hoc: Males Full vs Part p=.0155). K, BNST Tone response averaged from t=0-2 in Part fear across tone 3, 4 and 5 (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(2,34)=0.1031 ns, sex: F(1, 17)=12.05 p<.01, tone x sex: F(2, 34)=7.572 p<.01, Tukey post-hoc: Males tone 4 vs 5 p=.0147, Females tone 4 vs 5 p=.0423, Tone 3 Males vs Females p=0.0061, Tone 5 Males and Females p<.0001). L, Shock response in Part fear averaged from t=28-30 across all shock trials (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(2,34)=0.7578 ns, sex: F(1, 17)=3.788 ns p=.07, tone x sex: F(2, 34)=0.3663 ns). M, Shock omission response in Part fear averaged from t=28-30 across no shock trials (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(2,34)=0.8610 ns, sex: F(1, 17)=2.054 ns, tone x sex: F(2, 34)=1.689 ns). N, Fear recall traces in males. O, Fear recall traces in females. P, BNST Tone response averaged from t=0-2 for all tones in Full and Part fear recall (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,30)=0.02371 ns, sex: F(1, 30)=0.001158 ns, fear x sex: F(1, 30)=0.01792 ns). Q, Area under the curve (AUC) during tone presentations from t=0-30 (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,30)=0.7822 ns, sex: F(1, 30)=7.444 p<.05, fear x sex: F(1, 30)=2.036 ns). n=7-10/group. All data are shown as mean ± SEM. * = p<.05, ** = p<.01, *** = p<.001, **** = p<.0001.
First, we compared BNST responses to tone onset (first 2 seconds). Considering all tones, Part fear conditioning led to a greater tone response than Full fear, which post hoc testing suggests is driven by the much higher BNST tone response from Full fear males (Fig 2F). Interestingly, tone responses in males were also significantly higher than in females (Fig 2F). To minimize any differences driven by the number of tone presentations, we compared the responses to tone 3 in Full fear mice to the response to tone 4 in Part fear mice (Fig 2G). Here we find no differences in tone response between Full and Part fear, however, reduced engagement of the BNST in females regardless of fear type persists (Fig 2G).
Next, we quantified BNST responses to shock and found an interaction effect.
Post-hoc tests reveal that BNST responses to shock were higher in Part fear males than Full, and that in the Full fear group, shock responses were higher in females than in males (Fig 2I). Comparing the final shock only, Part fear males show a greater response than Full fear males, while no difference in response amplitude is present in females (Fig 2J).
To characterize how BNST activity changes within subjects as uncertainty is introduced, we compared responses to tone onset in Part fear mice to tones 3, 4, and 5. Interestingly, BNST responses in males increase between tones 4 (preceded by shock omission) and 5 (preceded by shock) (Fig 2K), suggesting that the expected likelihood of receiving a shock may influence BNST activity. Further, the opposite effect is present in females, with female BNST response decreasing between tones 4 and 5, suggesting sex differences in how the BNST processes uncertain threats. Responses to shock or shock omission did not differ across time or between sexes in part fear mice (Fig 2L, M). Interestingly, while females showed little BNST engagement to tone onset, BNST responses to shock and omission was similar to males (Fig 2L-M), indicating the sex differences exist primarily during the cue exposure in fear learning.
During fear recall, neither sex nor fear type affected BNST activity to tone onset (first 2 seconds of tone) (Fig 2N-P). However, we found that in males (Fig 2N), but not females (Fig 2O), BNST activity dipped below baseline during the full 30 sec tone presentations for both Full and Part fear (Fig 2Q). This suggests that sex differences to tone onset are only observed during fear conditioning and not recall, however, sex differences persist in BNST responses to the full tone exposure in recall. Overall, BNST dynamics were highly similar across groups and sexes during learning, and thus the subtle differences in signaling may communicate the nuanced distinctions between Full and Part fear.
CRF knockdown in the BNST potentiates partially reinforced fear acquisition and recall in females
CRF signaling in the BNST is critical for sustained fear6,7, but classic studies do not distinguish local CRF signaling from CRF inputs to the BNST. To determine the role of CRF in BNST neurons in Part fear, we used a viral approach to genetically knockdown CRF in the BNST33. To do this, we injected an AAV encoding cre recombinase or GFP bilaterally into the BNST of CRFlox/lox mice33 three weeks prior to behavior testing (Fig 3A-C). First, we compared freezing during fear conditioning and recall in male GFP and Cre mice. Part fear male mice regardless of virus froze more than Full fear mice when comparing all tone exposures (Fig 3D), but this effect did not appear when comparing just the final shocked tone exposure (Fig 3E). Like wildtype mice, male Part fear mice froze more during tone 5 in fear conditioning than tones 3 and 4 (Fig 3F). There were no differences in freezing during baseline (Fig 3G) or first tone exposure (Fig 3H) in recall between male GFP and Cre mice, suggesting that CRF knockdown in the BNST does not alter full or part fear learning or recall in males.

CRF knockdown in the BNST potentiates fear learning and recall in females.
A, Surgical schematic and timeline of CRF knockdown experiments. B,C, Representative image of Cre and GFP virus expression in BNST. D, Males percent freezing to all tone exposures during fear conditioning (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,36)=7.871 p<.01, virus: F(1, 36)=0.1492 ns, fear x virus: F(1, 36)=0.4452 ns). E, Comparison of males Full tone 3 and Part tone 4 freezing response (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,36)=0.04672 ns, virus: F(1, 36)=0.004876 ns, fear x virus: F(1, 36)=0.3099 ns). F, Males freezing response in Part fear across tone 3, 4 and 5 (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(1.899,36.08)=6.882 p<.01, virus: F(1, 19)=.0044 ns, tone x virus: F(2, 38)=0.5234 ns). G, Freezing during baseline period in males Full and Part fear recall. H, Freezing during first tone presentation in males Full and Part fear recall. I, Females percent freezing to all tone exposures during fear conditioning (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,32)=16.71 p<.001, virus: F(1, 32)=1.393 ns, fear x virus: F(1, 32)=0.4367 ns). J, Comparison of females Full tone 3 and Part tone 4 freezing response (two- way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,32)=5.265 p<.05, virus: F(1, 32)=0.8753 ns, fear x virus: F(1, 32)=0.3144 ns). K, Females freezing response in Part fear across tone 3, 4 and 5 (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(1.596,25.54)=3.840 p<.05, virus: F(1, 16)=5.768 p<.05, tone x virus: F(2, 32)=0.4641 ns). L, Freezing during baseline in females Full and Part fear recall (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,32)=0.3949 ns, virus: F(1, 32)=8.591 p<.01, fear x virus: F(1, 32)=4.283 p<0.05, Bonferroni’s post-hoc: Part GFP vs Cre p=.0025). M, Freezing during first tone presentation in females Full and Part fear recall (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,32)=1.628 ns, virus: F(1, 32)=4.545 p<.05, fear x virus: F(1, 32)=1.886 ns). All data are shown as mean ± SEM. * = p<.05, ** = p<.01, *** = p<.001, **** = p<.0001.000
In females, Part fear mice also freeze more than Full fear mice when comparing all tone exposures (Fig 3I), and interestingly, this effect persists when comparing just the last shocked tone (Fig 3J). Though CRF knockdown slightly increased freezing in Part fear only, this effect was not significant. However, comparisons of freezing across tones in just Part fear females reveals a main effect of CRF knockdown, where knockdown increases freezing across tones (Fig 3K). Interestingly, CRF knockdown selectively increased freezing in Part Fear mice during the baseline period of fear recall (Fig 3L). Further, CRF knockdown also led to heightened freezing during recall first tone presentation regardless of fear type (Fig 3M). Overall, CRF knockdown led to an increase in fear learning in Part fear females (Fig 3K) and in fear recall in both Full and Part fear mice that appears to be driven by more pronounced changes in Part fear mice (Fig 3L-M). These results indicate that CRF knockdown in the BNST drives fear generalization and increased fear memory after partially reinforced fear in females, and that CRF differentially shapes passive coping in males and females.
CRF knockdown in the BNST does not modulate anxiety-like or vigilance behaviors after fear
Next, we asked whether CRF knockdown alters anxiety-like and vigilance behaviors. Using the same approach described above, we knocked down CRF in the BNST and examined changes in anxiety-like behavior (Fig 4A). We found that CRF knockdown did not alter latency to feed in the NSF assay in male (Fig 4B) and female mice (Fig 4C), but both Full and Part fear conditioning led to a significant reduction in latency to feed compared to Ctrl mice in both sexes. Importantly, there was no effect of CRF knockdown on the amount of food consumed during the refeed portion of the test in any group, indicating that this approach does not alter consummatory behavior (Fig 4D-E). In the acoustic startle test, CRF knockdown did not affect startle amplitude in either sex (Fig 4F-J). Although there were no sex differences in startle amplitude in Ctrl mice, males had greater startle amplitude than females after both Full fear (Fig 4G) and Part fear (Fig 4H). To compare fear types, we separated males and females and focused on the loudest startle stimulus (120dB) and found that fear conditioning potentiated startle amplitude in males (Fig 4I) but not females (Fig 4J). This is like what was found in Fig 1, except that male Part fear mice did not exhibit a robust increase. Overall, these findings suggest that CRF knockdown in the BNST does not shape these avoidance or arousal behaviors.

CRF knockdown in the BNST does not modulate anxiety-like or vigilance behaviors after fear.
A, Surgical schematic and behavioral timeline. B, Latency to feed in the novelty-induced suppression of feeding (NSF) test in males (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(2,46)=15.24 p<.0001, main effect of virus: F(1,46)=1.808 ns, fear x virus interaction F(2,46)=2.972 ns p=.06). C, Latency to feed in the NSF test in females (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear F(2,39)=1.190 p<.05, main effect of virus F(1,39)=1.190 ns, fear x virus interaction F(2,39)=0.1847 ns). D, Amount consumed during refeed test in males. E, Amount consumed during refeed test in females. F, Acoustic startle response in Ctrl mice (three-way RM ANOVA, main effect of dB: F(1.101,30.83)=27.40 p<.0001, virus: F(1, 28)=.0298 ns, sex: F(1, 28)=0.0167 ns). G, Acoustic startle response in Full fear mice (three-way RM ANOVA, main effect of dB: F(1.701,49.33)=126.3 p<.0001, virus: F(1, 29)=3.590 ns p=.07, sex: F(1, 29)=5.260 p<.05, dB x Sex F(2, 58)=5.717 p<.01). H, Acoustic startle response in Part fear mice (three-way RM ANOVA, main effect of dB: F(1.466,41.05)=88.49 p<.0001, virus: F(1, 28)=.0037 ns, sex: F(1, 28)=4.763 p<.05, dB x Sex F(2, 56)=3.965 p<.05). I, Acoustic startle response at 120dB in males (two-way ANOVA, main effect of main effect of fear: F(2,46)= 12.96 p<.0001, main effect of virus: F(1,46)=1.131 ns, fear x virus interaction: F(2,46)=1.666 ns). J, Acoustic startle response at 120dB in females (two-way ANOVA, main effect of main effect of fear: F(2,39)= 2.046 ns, main effect of virus: F(1,39)=0.0080 ns, fear x virus interaction: F(2,39)=0.0592 ns). N = 7-10/group. All data are shown as mean ± SEM. * = p<.05, ** = p<.01, *** = p<.001, **** = p<.0001.
Activity of BNST CRF neurons during fear
We previously found that BNSTCRF neurons contribute to fear encoding34 but only when serotonin levels are increased in the BNST. This suggests that in some situations, BNSTCRF can directly regulate fear learning. Given the effects of CRF knockdown on fear learning and recall in female mice (Fig 3), we hypothesized that males and females had differential engagement of BNSTCRF during fear conditioning and recall, which could drive the behavioral consequences of fear learning. To record BNSTCRF activity, we injected an AAV encoding a cre-dependent GCaMP8m into the BNST of CRF-cre mice and implanted GRIN lenses for 1-photon calcium imaging (Fig 5A).

CRF neurons differentially encode fear recall in males and females.
A, Viral strategy and experimental design schematic. B, Example traces of different responses to foot shock. C, Pie charts showing the proportion of cells excited and inhibited in response to the first foot shock in males and females, fear groups combined. D, Event frequency during baseline period in fear conditioning baseline (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,161)=0.00030 ns, sex: F(1, 161)=7.558 p<.01, fear x sex: F(1, 161)=3.987 p<.05). E, Event frequency as a change from baseline during consolidation period at the end of fear conditioning (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,161)=0.3744 ns, sex: F(1, 161)=18.91 p<.0001, fear x sex: F(1, 161)=0.6552 ns). F, Event frequency during all tone presentations in fear conditioning (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,161)=0.1263 ns, sex: F(1, 161)=6.462 p<.05, fear x sex: F(1, 161)=7.135 p<.01, Bonferroni Post Hoc: Full, Males vs Females p=0.0011). G, Event frequency during final shock tone, Full fear tone 3 and Part fear tone 4 (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,161)=0.2.638 ns, sex: F(1, 161)=2.060 ns, fear x sex: F(1, 161)=0.02164 ns). H, final shock in Full and Part fear conditioning (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,161)=1.377 ns, sex: F(1, 161)=0.9991 ns, fear x sex: F(1, 161)=1.125 ns). I, Example traces during baseline and trial end in fear recall. J, Full and Part fear recall baseline period event frequency (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,162)=0.05488 ns, sex: F(1, 162)=0.2847 ns, fear x sex: F(1, 162)=0.0058 ns). K, Full and Part fear recall trial end event frequency as a change from baseline (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,162)=8.357 p<.01, sex: F(1, 162)=4.143 p<.05, fear x sex: F(1, 162)=0.1101 ns, Bonferroni’s post-hoc: Males Full vs Part p=.0384). L, Event frequency during all tone presentations in recall (two-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,162)=4.593 p<.05, sex: F(1, 162)=3.202 ns p=.08, fear x sex: F(1, 162)=0.0013 ns). M, Comparison of event frequency during first and last tone presentation in recall (three-way ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,162)=10.85 p<.01, sex: F(1, 162)=7.385 p<.01, fear x sex: F(1, 162)=3.981 p<.05, fear x sex x tone: F(1,162)=5.916 p<.05, Tukey’s post hoc: Males Part 1st tone vs Males Part last tone p=.0004, Males Part last tone vs Females Part last tone p=.0005). N, Comparison of event frequency during the 10 seconds following the first and last tone presentation in recall (three-way ANOVA, main effect of sex: F(1, 162)=3.340 ns p=.07, fear x sex: F(1, 162)=20.62 p<.0001, fear x sex x tone: F(1,162)=4.334 p<.05, Tukey’s post hoc: Males Full 1st tone vs Males Full last tone p=.0069, Males Full 1st tone vs Females full 1st tone p=.0237, Females Full last tone vs Females Part 1st tone p = .0253). For fear conditioning: Full Males: n=41 cells from 6 mice. Part Males: n=39 cells from 5 mice. Full females: n= 33 cells from 6 mice. Part females: n=52 cells from 5 mice. For fear recall: Full Males: n=45 cells from 6 mice. Part Males: n=44 cells from 5 mice. Full females: n= 33 cells from 6 mice. Part females: n=45 cells from 5 mice. All data are shown as mean ± SEM. * = p<.05, ** = p<.01, *** = p<.001, **** = p<.0001.
First, we compared BNSTCRF activity between Full and Part fear conditioning and recall. During fear conditioning, we observed that roughly a third of cells were excited by shock in both sexes, and the rest were inhibited (Fig 5B-C). The BNST is sexually dimorphic, and females have more BNSTCRF neurons than males25,26, therefore we first examined if there were any baseline differences between male and female BNSTCRF activity. We compared BNSTCRF activity during the baseline period in fear conditioning, before any shock or tone exposures, and found that female BNSTCRF neurons were more active than males (Fig 5D). Interestingly, activity decreased throughout the conditioning session for both sexes, but the reduction was more pronounced in females, as reveled by comparison of activity during the consolidation period of fear conditioning (last 2 minutes of trial) to baseline (Fig 5E). When comparing BNSTCRF event frequency during tone exposures, we found that females, regardless of fear group had higher activity than males (Fig 5F). This could be due to females having higher activity overall, in the BNSTCRF neurons. In fact, when comparing the last tone shock between Part and Full fear, we find no significant effects of sex or fear conditioning type (Fig 5G). To characterize BNSTCRF responses to shock, we compared the 10 seconds following the final shock and found no differences in event frequency in BNSTCRF between Full or Part fear or between sexes (Fig 5H).
Differences in expression of fear during recall between fear groups and sexes point to sex specific roles of BNSTCRF neurons during fear recall. Interestingly, there were no sex differences in event frequency during the baseline period in recall (Fig 5J), but similar to conditioning, BNSTCRF activity decreased significantly between baseline and session end during recall (Fig 5K). However, Part fear male BNSTCRF activity showed a smaller reduction compared to Full fear males (Fig 5I & K), suggesting that male BNSTCRF neurons are more engaged during recall of uncertain fear. This remains true when looking at the first and last tone exposure, where Part fear males showed an increase in Event frequency from first to last tone compared to Full fear males and females (Fig 5M). Further, elevated engagement of BNSTCRF neurons was also seen in Part fear mice regardless of sex during tone exposure compared to Full fear mice (Fig 5L). Interestingly, comparisons of activity during the 10 seconds after tone end (when mice might expect to receive a shock) reveal that males show a reduction in activity between first and last tone regardless of fear group, while females show an increase in BNSTCRF activity (Fig 5N).
Further, we noted sex differences in Part fear conditioning (Fig 6). BNSTCRF event frequency during tones 3, 4, and 5 differed between sexes; while male mice showed similar levels of BNST event frequency during tones, BNSTCRF event frequency decreased across tone exposures in females (Fig 6E). This effect is similar to what we saw with general BNST activity in fiber photometry recordings (Fig 2) and suggests that different neural processes underly sustained fear encoding in females. Half of the tones in Part fear conditioning terminate in shock while the other half do not (shock omission), thus we compared event frequency in the10 second window following a shock or omission trial (Fig 6F-I). Both males and females demonstrated a slight increase in activity during the second shock (Fig 6F). Interestingly, while males had some BNSTCRF activity after shock omission, female mice showed little engagement of BNSTCRF neurons following each omission trial (Fig 6G). To compare BNSTCRF activity post shock or omission, we examined only the final shock and omission trials (Fig 6H). Male BNSTCRF neurons are more engaged during omission than shock, while female BNST is more engaged during shock than omission (Fig 6H-I). Overall, these data demonstrate sex differences in BNSTCRF activity during fear conditioning, where females show greater silencing of BNSTCRF neurons compared to males. This may also indicate sex differences in how BNSTCRF encode negative prediction error during Part fear conditioning.

Sex differences in BNSTCRF neuron encoding of Part fear conditioning.
A, Experimental design schematic for 1-photon recordings. B, representative raw FOV. C, representative processed FOV. D, representative image showing GCaMP expression and GRIN lens placement. E, Event frequency during partial fear tone 3, 4, and 5 presentations (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of tone: F(2,180)=7.330 p<.001, sex: F(1, 90)=0.3467 ns, tone x sex: F(2, 180)=6.365 p<.01, Bonferroni’s post-hoc: Females tone 3 vs 4 p<.0001, Females tone 3 vs 5 p<.0001). F, Event frequency during the 10 s post-shock across all shock trials (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of shock: F(2,178)=5.386 p<.01, sex: F(1, 89)=0.0044 ns, shock x sex: F(2, 178)=1.243 ns). G, Event frequency during the 10 s post-omission across all omission trials (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of omission: F(2,178)=1.412 ns, sex: F(1, 89)=8.552 p<.01, omission x sex: F(2, 178)=0.3291 ns). H, Event frequency during the 10 s final post-shock or post-omission window during partial fear (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of shock/omission: F(1,89)=.0209 ns, sex: F(1, 89)=3.174 ns p=.08, shock/omission x sex: F(1, 89)=4.697 p<.05, Bonferroni’s post-hoc: Omission Males vs Females p=.0135). I, Representative traces showing a response to shock and shock-omission trials for partial fear only in males and females. All data are shown as mean ± SEM. * = p<.05, ** = p<.01, *** = p<.001, **** = p<.0001.
Discussion
Partially Reinforced Fear Conditioning Drives Sustained Fear in Males
Phasic fear refers to defensive responses triggered by clear and imminent threats, which dissipate rapidly once the threat is removed7. In contrast, sustained fear is characterized by a prolonged state of apprehension, driven by distant or ambiguous cues that persist over time. Full fear and Part fear conditioning are rodent experimental paradigms designed to model phasic and sustained fear, respectively. Both consist of three tone-shock pairings, but Part fear includes an additional three tones that do not co-terminate with a shock (Fig. 1A). This small difference in the Part fear paradigm does not consistently drive distinct behavioral effects in mice. Although we observed differences in freezing when comparing all tone exposures, these are likely due to Part fear consisting of three more tones, as there were no significant differences between Full fear and Part fear groups during specific tone exposures in fear conditioning and recall.
The Part fear paradigm allows for careful dissection of uncertain fear learning by allowing for within-mouse comparisons before and after uncertainty are introduced. A notable and consistent finding was an increase in freezing behavior during tone 5 presentation in the Part fear group compared to tones 3 and 4 (Figs. 1 & 3). This pattern offers insights into sustained fear encoding, as it highlights how uncertainty can shape defensive behaviors. As tones 1 and 2 co-terminate with shock, mice are likely to perceive the third tone as a threat cue. Tone 4 introduces uncertainty, as it follows a tone that did not co-terminate with a shock. By tone 5, the ambiguity is heightened, as mice have now experienced both tone-shock pairings and tone-shock omissions, making tone 5 the first truly ambiguous tone in the Part fear paradigm. This escalation in ambiguity likely underpins the observed increase in freezing and provides a valuable model for studying sustained fear.
Our findings demonstrate that fear conditioning increases the acoustic startle response in males only (Figs. 1 & 4). In wild-type mice, this heightened startle response is driven by a pronounced increase in hypervigilance in males who underwent Part fear conditioning (Fig. 1R). Notably, this effect was observed three days after fear learning, indicating a sustained increase in arousal. The acoustic startle test serves as a valuable measure of psychomotor arousal, and elevated startle responses are commonly seen in individuals with anxiety disorders35,36. Hypervigilance and heightened threat responsiveness are also core symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in humans35,37,38. Interestingly, a history of fear conditioning did not lead to any significant behavioral differences in females, either in the NSF (novelty-suppressed feeding) test or in startle amplitude. This highlights a significant gap in the literature, as studies exploring the effects of fear exposure in females remain limited. Further research is essential to understand the factors driving behavioral responses in females after fear conditioning, especially since NSF and startle are not the only tests measuring aspects of anxiety and vigilance. Exploring additional behavioral paradigms will be critical to uncovering the nuances of fear responses in females.
BNST Dynamics During Fear
While classical fear literature emphasizes the amygdala’s critical role in phasic fear and the BNST’s involvement in sustained fear, recent findings suggest that the BNST contributes to both fear types but is more prominently recruited and indispensable10 for sustained fear. For example, a recent neuroimaging study in humans found that the BNST showed greater engagement than the amygdala during ambiguous threats, highlighting its role in processing sustained fear across species12. Accordingly, we investigated the role of BNST neurons in both phasic and sustained fear in male and female mice using fiber photometry to measure BNST population activity (Fig. 2). Our data support the hypothesis that the BNST is more responsive to uncertain fear cues in males, but not females. This finding is critical, as few studies have examined sex differences in BNST responses, despite the higher prevalence of anxiety and mood disorders related to sustained fear, such as PTSD, in women.
In male mice, BNST activity was significantly higher in response to tone 5 compared to tones 3 and 4 during Part fear condition (Fig. 2K), and BNST tone responses were highest in Part fear males (Fig. 2F). This may represent that the BNST encodes the expected likelihood of a negative outcome. This is consistent with in vivo electrophysiology recordings suggesting that roughly 40% of BNST neurons encode attention or uncertainty of aversive contingency39. Further, human fMRI studies have linked BNST activity to anticipation of threat, particularly those that are ambiguous. We also found that male Part fear mice exhibited the highest BNST response to the final shock compared to Full fear males and females (Fig. 2J). These findings indicate that BNST activity is particularly heightened in males during sustained fear and match the increased vigilance observed behaviorally (Fig. 1). In contrast, female mice showed minimal BNST responses to tone presentations during fear conditioning (Figs. 2F-H & K). Notably, in Part fear, females exhibited a reduction, rather than the expected increase, in BNST activity in response to tone 5 (Fig. 2K). Importantly, these sex differences were specific to tone presentation, as BNST activity did not differ between sexes during shock (Fig. 2L) or shock omission (Fig. 2M) in Part fear.
Sex differences in BNST dynamics also emerged during fear recall, where we showed that BNST activity was overall suppressed during the full 30 sec tones in males but not in females, with a slightly greater suppression in Part fear males versus Full fear (Fig 2Q). This is consistent with findings that an inhibitory input to the BNST from the medial prefrontal cortex is engaged during Part fear recall8. Additionally, during both conditioning and recall, activity in the BNST negatively correlated with freezing (Fig S1), suggesting that this is an innate property of the BNST and is not altered by fear conditioning. These results are consistent with findings that the BNST mediates the expression of proactive avoidance behaviors to prevent harm40. Interestingly, the inhibition of a specific BLA-BNST pathway reduces fear conditioning to a sustained cue in males but not females, suggesting that different BNST subpopulations may mediate sex-specific responses41. These results emphasize the importance of studying BNST microcircuits and their contributions to fear processing across sexes.
CRF knockdown alters fear learning and expression in females only
The BNST and CRF signaling are critical for sustained fear across species7,14,22,25,42. However, the mechanisms through which CRF in the BNST contributes to fear expression remain unclear. The BNST contains CRF-expressing neurons that release CRF locally and to other brain regions, and receives CRF inputs from external sources34,43. These distinct CRF nodes may differentially influence fear expression. Studies examining sustained fear in rodents point towards distinct roles for CRF inputs to the BNST and BNSTCRF neurons. CRF infusions into the BNST increase fear expression14, while CRF receptor antagonism reduces sustained fear expression44, suggesting that CRF inputs to the BNST are crucial for sustained fear expression. Conversely, one study using viral overexpression of CRF in the BNST of males found that it weakened sustained fear expression15, which suggests that CRF inputs to the BNST may drive sustained fear while exaggerated BNSTCRF signaling may attenuate sustained fear. These studies have been done in males only; thus we focused on understanding how CRF knockdown in BNSTCRF neurons affects behavior in males and females using a genetic knockdown approach (Fig 3-4).
Since CRF overexpression weakened sustained fear in male rats, we hypothesized that CRF knockdown would enhance sustained fear in male mice, however, we found that CRF knockdown in the BNST led to no changes in Full or Part fear conditioning or recall in males (Fig 3D-H). This may be due to a species difference or a difference in the sustained fear paradigm used. The partially reinforced cued fear paradigm used in these studies may not be salient or different enough from a fully reinforced fear paradigm to reliably elicit behavioral differences and future studies using more prolonged sustained fear paradigms may be more informative. This is supported by the evidence that BNSTCRF signaling is not necessary for phasic cued fear conditioning, as chemogenetic inhibition of male BNSTCRF neurons during conditioning or consolidation of cued fear conditioning has no effect on fear34,45. Interestingly, CRF knockdown in females led to a significant increase in freezing behavior across tones in Part fear conditioning (Fig 3K) and an increase in freezing during baseline and the first tone presentation during recall in both Part and Full fear (Fig 3L-M). These effects appear driven by Part fear females, as post-hoc tests show that knockdown led to generalized fear expression in Part fear female mice compared to GFP controls (Fig 3L). This suggests that CRF knockdown only impacts fear in female mice, driving a general increase in fear learning and expression.
Intra-BNST infusion of CRF potentiates acoustic startle response46, and locally- projecting BNST CRF neurons drive anxiety by inhibiting anxiolytic outputs from BNST34,43. Thus, we hypothesized that knockdown of CRF in the BNST would block the increased acoustic startle we observed in males after fear (Fig 1N-R). However, CRF knockdown had no effect in the acoustic startle test or the NSF assay. CRF inputs to the BNST are also important for fear related behaviors. It may be that behavioral effects in the startle test or NSF are instead driven by CRF inputs to the BNST, thus, future experiments knocking down CRF R1 and R2 will clarify these effects. BNSTCRF neurons project both locally and out of the BNST34,43, which may indicate that CRF signaling within the BNST versus in target regions of BNSTCRF differentially alters acoustic startle reflex. Alternatively, the lack of an effect of CRF knockdown on acoustic startle after fear suggests a potential CRF-independent role of BNSTCRF neurons instead, as this population also releases GABA47. This is especially interesting, as we recently found that knockdown of GABA vs CRF BNSTCRF neurons led to differential regulation of operant ethanol self-administration48. Overall, these findings are consistent with reports that CRF plays divergent roles in anxiety-like and stress behaviors in males and females22,25,49.
BNSTCRF neurons are persistently active during Part fear expression in males
Given the established link between CRF levels in the BNST and sustained fear, we hypothesized that BNSTCRF neuron activity would be elevated after Part, but not Full fear conditioning. Unexpectedly, we found minimal differences in BNSTCRF spiking between fear groups in 1-photon calcium imaging experiments. However, these results align with our previous findings showing that chemogenetic inhibition of CRF neurons does not affect Full fear responses34, and suggest that CRF neuron activity during fear conditioning does not play a crucial role in the distinction between phasic and sustained fear.
Interestingly, male Part fear mice demonstrated persistent engagement of BNSTCRF neurons during the recall baseline through the end of the session (Fig. 5I). They also exhibited increased engagement during the last tone compared to the first tone in recall (Fig. 5M). Persistent BNSTCRF activity is particularly interesting, as elevated central CRF levels and CRF signaling hyperactivity have been documented in people with stress- related disorders50. Accordingly, elevated BNST CRF neuron activity may drive the behavioral alterations observed following Part fear, like the hypervigilance observed in Figure 1.
Consistent with other studies19, we found that females overall had higher basal BNSTCRF tone than males (Fig 5D). In Figure 6, we observed that female BNSTCRF spiking decreased during tones 4, and 5 in Part fear (Fig 6E). Additionally, while male BNSTCRF neurons exhibited higher event frequency following shock omission than after shock trials, female BNSTCRF activity was suppressed after omission trials (Fig 6F-I), suggesting reduced engagement during ambiguity in females. These findings suggest nuanced sex- and condition-dependent roles of BNSTCRF neurons in fear processing.
Notably, we did not observe major sex differences in BNSTCRF activity during fear recall despite robust behavioral differences after CRF knockdown. There are a few possible explanations for this. First, Ca2+ activity in CRF cell bodies may not necessarily reflect release of CRF itself. Others have shown that regulation of dopamine terminals leads to changes in dopamine release that do not reflect cell body activity51; thus, it is possible that there are sex differences in CRF release that we cannot capture using this method. Second, CRF signaling in the BNST drives distinct behavioral and cellular responses to stress in male and female rats26–28. Thus, sex differences in behavior may emerge downstream of BNSTCRF neuron activity.
Further, it is important to note that BNSTCRF neurons comprise two dissociable populations with opposing roles in fear processing. CRF interneurons projecting within the BNST inhibit anxiolytic and fear-dampening outputs to areas such as the ventral tegmental area and lateral hypothalamus, thereby driving fear and anxiety 4,43. Consequently, recording activity from all BNSTCRF neurons may obscure critical changes occurring within specific subpopulations. Further studies aimed at disentangling the contributions of these distinct populations during partially reinforced fear are needed.
Conclusions
Previous studies have not extensively explored sex differences in neural responses to uncertain threat, and our findings reveal critical sex differences in BNST function and behavior during sustained fear. Males exhibited hypervigilance and heightened BNST engagement with increasing ambiguity, while females show reduced BNST engagement. Additionally, knockdown of CRF in BNSTCRF neurons led to increased fear expression in female mice only, suggesting that CRF in the BNST is important for the normal expression of fear in females. The consistent blunting of BNST population activity (Fig 2) and single cell BNSTCRF activity (Fig 5-6) during tone exposures in Part fear females are particularly compelling, as women are more likely to develop PTSD, a condition associated with impaired discrimination between threat and safety cues38. These findings suggest potential innate sex differences in BNST- mediated threat cue discrimination, which may drive sex-specific vulnerabilities to affective mood disorders. These differences provide insight into the neural circuits underlying fear and stress disorders, particularly in females, who face a higher prevalence of anxiety and mood disorders. Future research focusing on BNST microcircuits and sex-specific adaptations is essential to develop targeted interventions.

BNST activity negatively correlates with freezing.
A Schematic of viral infection. B. Representative timeline showing freeze and mobile spiking of BNST. C, Freezing during conditioning in males D, Freezing during recall in males. E, BNST spiking frequency during mobile/freeze mobility states in Full and Part fear conditioning males (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,15)=2.295 ns, mobility: F(1, 15)=72.38 p<.0001, fear x mobility: F(1, 15)=5.358 p<.05, Bonferroni’s post-hoc: Full Mobile vs Freeze p=.0008, Part Mobile vs Freeze p<.0001, Mobile Full vs Part p=.0287). F, BNST spiking frequency during recall mobile/freeze mobility states in males (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,15)=.6208 ns, mobility: F(1, 15)=61.20 p<.0001, fear x mobility: F(1, 15)=1.427 ns). G, Freezing during conditioning in females H, Freezing during recall in females. I, BNST spiking frequency during mobile/freeze mobility states in Full and Part fear conditioning females (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,15)=0.3466 ns, mobility: F(1, 15)=49.44 p<.0001, fear x mobility: F(1, 15)=0.0303 ns).). J, BNST spiking frequency during recall mobile/freeze mobility states in females (two-way RM ANOVA, main effect of fear: F(1,15)=0.0057 ns, mobility: F(1, 15)=17.79 p<.001, fear x mobility: F(1, 15)=6.095 p<.05, Bonferroni’s post-hoc: Part Mobile vs Freeze p=.0004).
References
- 1.Adaptations to Predators and PreyIn: The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology John Wiley & Sons, Ltd pp. 200–223https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939376.ch7
- 2.Risk assessment as an evolved threat detection and analysis processNeurosci. Biobehav. Rev 35:991–998
- 3.A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: Predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behaviorIn: Evolution and learning Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc pp. 185–212
- 4.Antipredator responses and defensive behavior: ecological and ethological approaches for the neurosciencesNeurosci. Biobehav. Rev 25:577–586
- 5.The ecology of human fear: survival optimization and the nervous systemFront. Neurosci 9
- 6.Amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis: differential roles in fear and anxiety measured with the acoustic startle reflexPhilos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci 352:1675–1687
- 7.Phasic vs Sustained Fear in Rats and Humans: Role of the Extended Amygdala in Fear vs AnxietyNeuropsychopharmacology 35:105–135
- 8.A prefrontal-bed nucleus of the stria terminalis circuit limits fear to uncertain threateLife 9:e60812
- 9.Role of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in aversive learning and memoryLearn. Mem 24:480–491
- 10.Bed nucleus of the stria terminalis regulates fear to unpredictable threat signalseLife 8:e46525
- 11.Sex Differences in BNST and Amygdala Activation by Contextual, Cued, and Unpredictable ThreatseNeuro 9
- 12.Explicit and Ambiguous Threat Processing: Functionally Dissociable Roles of the Amygdala and Bed Nucleus of the Stria TerminalisJ. Cogn. Neurosci 31:543–559
- 13.Posttraining infusion of norepinephrine and corticotropin releasing factor into the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis enhanced retention in an inhibitory avoidance taskChin. J. Physiol 44:33–43
- 14.Differential effects of the CRF-R1 antagonist GSK876008 on fear- potentiated, light- and CRF-enhanced startle suggest preferential involvement in sustained vs phasic threat responsesNeuropsychopharmacol. Off. Publ. Am. Coll. Neuropsychopharmacol 34:1533–1542
- 15.Effects of continuously enhanced corticotropin releasing factor expression within the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis on conditioned and unconditioned anxietyMol. Psychiatry 18:308–319
- 16.Gender Differences in Anxiety Disorders: Prevalence, Course of Illness, Comorbidity and Burden of IllnessJ. Psychiatr. Res 45:1027–1035
- 17.A comparison of mental health symptomatology and levels of social support in young treatment seeking transgender individuals who identify as binary and non-binaryInt. J. Transgenderism 20:241–250
- 18.The bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in the rat: regional sex differences controlled by gonadal steroids early after birthBrain Res 429:295–300
- 19.The paraventricular thalamus provides a polysynaptic brake on limbic CRF neurons to sex-dependently blunt binge alcohol drinking and avoidance behavior in miceNat. Commun 12:5080
- 20.Sex differences in PTSD resilience and susceptibility: Challenges for animal models of fear learningNeurobiol. Stress 1:60–65
- 21.Sex differences, gonadal hormones and the fear extinction network: implications for anxiety disordersBiol. Mood Anxiety Disord 2
- 22.Sex differences in stress responses: a critical role for corticotropin-releasing factorHorm. Athens Greece 17:5–13
- 23.Stressor-specific effects of sex on HPA axis hormones and activation of stress-related neurocircuitryStress Amst. Neth 16:664–677
- 24.Increased CRF mRNA expression in the sexually dimorphic BNST of male but not female GAD67 mice and TMT predator odor stress effects upon spatial memory retrievalBehav. Brain Res 272:141–149
- 25.Sex differences in circuits activated by corticotropin releasing factor in ratsHorm. Behav 97:145–153
- 26.Distinct populations of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) neurons mediate divergent yet complementary defensive behaviors in response to a threatNeuropharmacology 228
- 27.Female-biased sexual dimorphism of corticotropin-releasing factor neurons in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalisBiol. Sex Differ 10
- 28.Multi-Fiber Photometry to Record Neural Activity in Freely-Moving AnimalsJoVE J. Vis. Exp :e60278https://doi.org/10.3791/60278
- 29.DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learningNat. Neurosci 21:1281–1289
- 30.Simple Behavioral Analysis (SimBA) – an open source toolkit for computer classification of complex social behaviors in experimental animalsbioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.19.049452
- 31.Anxious Responses to Predictable and Unpredictable Aversive EventsBehav. Neurosci 118:916–924
- 32.Role of bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and amygdala AMPA receptors in the development and expression of context conditioning and sensitization of startle by prior shockBrain Struct. Funct 219:1969–1982
- 33.Corticotropin-releasing factor neurons in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis exhibit sex-specific pain encoding in miceSci. Rep 11:12500
- 34.Serotonin engages an anxiety and fear-promoting circuit in the extended amygdalaNature 537:97–101
- 35.Pavlovian fear memory circuits and phenotype models of PTSDNeuropharmacology 62:638–646
- 36.Lesions of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis block sensitization of the acoustic startle reflex produced by repeated stress, but not fear-potentiated startleProg. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 22:625–648
- 37.Reduced Freezing in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Patients while Watching Affective PicturesFront. Psychiatry 8
- 38.Post-traumatic stress disorder: clinical and translational neuroscience from cells to circuitsNat. Rev. Neurol 18:273–288
- 39.Phasic signaling in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis during fear learning predicts within- and across-session cued fear expressionLearn. Mem. Cold Spring Harb. N 27:83–90
- 40.The bed nucleus of the stria terminalis mediates the expression of proactive defensive behaviorbioRxiv https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2022.08.16.504160v1
- 41.Sex Differences in the Activity of Basolateral Amygdalar Neurons That Project to the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis and Their Role in Anticipatory AnxietyJ. Neurosci 42:4488–4504
- 42.Human bed nucleus of the stria terminalis indexes hypervigilant threat monitoringBiol. Psychiatry 68:416–424
- 43.Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure dysregulates a GABAergic microcircuit in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalisNeuropharmacology 168:107759
- 44.Role of corticotropin releasing factor in anxiety disorders: A translational research perspectiveHorm. Behav 50:550–561
- 45.Somatostatin Neurons of the Bed Nucleus of Stria Terminalis Enhance Associative Fear Memory Consolidation in MiceJ. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 41:1982–1995
- 46.Role of the Hippocampus, the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis, and the Amygdala in the Excitatory Effect of Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone on the Acoustic Startle ReflexJ. Neurosci 17:6434–6446
- 47.Neuroanatomical evidence for reciprocal regulation of the corticotrophin-releasing factor and oxytocin systems in the hypothalamus and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis of the rat: Implications for balancing stress and affectPsychoneuroendocrinology 36:1312–1326
- 48.Disentangling the Effects of Corticotrophin Releasing Factor and GABA Release from the Ventral Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis on Ethanol Self-Administration in MicebioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.03.02.530838
- 49.Sex differences in molecular and cellular substrates of stressCell. Mol. Neurobiol 32:709–723
- 50.Region-specific roles of the corticotropin-releasing factor–urocortin system in stressNat. Rev. Neurosci 17:636–651
- 51.Axonal mechanisms mediating γ-aminobutyric acid receptor type A (GABA-A) inhibition of striatal dopamine releaseeLife 9:e55729
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
Copyright
© 2023, Hon et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 465
- downloads
- 10
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.