Abstract
Women are particularly underrepresented in journals of the highest scientific impact, with substantial consequences for their careers. While a large body of research has focused on the outcome and the process of peer review, fewer articles have explicitly focused on gendered submission behavior and the explanations for these differences. In our study of nearly five thousand active authors, we find that women are less likely to report having submitted papers and, when they have, to submit fewer manuscripts, on average, than men. Women were more likely to indicate that they did not submit their papers (in general and their subsequently most cited papers) to Science, Nature, or PNAS because they were advised not to. In the aggregate, no statistically significant difference was observed between men and women in how they rated the quality of their work. Nevertheless, regardless of discipline, women were more likely than men to indicate that their “work was not ground-breaking or sufficiently novel” as a rationale for not submitting to one of the listed prestigious journals. Men were more likely than women to indicate that the “work would fit better in a more specialized journal.” We discuss the implications of these findings and interventions that can serve to mitigate the disparities caused by gendered differences in submission behavior.
Significance
Publishing in high-impact scholarly journals has a significant effect on researchers’ careers. Our findings identify factors that affect submission to Science, Nature, and the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS) and explore whether there is a relationship between gender and desk rejections or submission rates. We found no relationship between gender and reported desk rejection and a relationship between gender and reported submissions. Women were more likely than men to indicate that their “work was not ground-breaking or sufficiently novel” for the listed prestigious journals and that they were advised against submitting to these venues. Men were more likely to indicate that the “work would fit better in a more specialized journal.”
Introduction
The rise of the research evaluation system has created a market of intense competition for a few hallowed venues. Scientific capital is strongly concentrated in generalist journals founded in the late nineteenth and early twentieth Centuries (Baldwin, 2015), such as Science, Nature, and Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS). Articles published in these journals garner greater news media attention, and authors tend to receive more career opportunities and grants (Reich, 2013). Since publishing in highly influential journals leads to wider dissemination and visibility of researchers’ work, the volume of manuscript submissions to these journals is high and acceptance low, with a large proportion of manuscripts rejected by editors without undergoing peer review. It is noted on Science’s author portal that less than 7% of originally submitted papers are accepted (Science, 2023). Nature also notes low acceptance rates on its website, from 8-12% depending on the year, and states that “many submissions are declined without being sent for review” (Nature, 2023). A similar practice is espoused by the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), with most publications rejected before peer review and a final acceptance rate of 14% (PNAS, 2023). Prestige and hierarchy among journals are not inherently negative for scholarly communication; however, if entry into these highly visible venues varies for different sociodemographic populations of authors, it may have adverse consequences for science (Kozlowski et al., 2022).
Editors and peer reviewers evaluate both the quality of manuscripts submitted as well as whether the topic fits within the scope of the journal. Idealized gatekeeping reflects the Mertonian norms of organized skepticism and universalism, evaluating scientific work independently of the social characteristics of authors (Merton, 1968). However, various critiques of scholarly peer review have been discussed (Ware, 2008), among which is a concern of social biases, that is, “differential evaluation of an author’s submission as a result of her/his perceived membership in a particular social category” (Lee et al., 2013). These biases are not necessarily conscious; yet, if no biases were present in the case of peer review, then “we should expect the rate with which members of less powerful social groups enjoy successful peer review outcomes to be proportionate to their representation in submission rates” (Lee et al., 2013). Results on gender disparities and bias in scholarly publishing have arrived at mixed results (Borsuk et al., 2009; Day et al., 2020; Djupe et al., 2019; Edwards et al., 2018; Fox & Paine, 2019; Gilbert et al., 1994; Grossman, 2020; Helmer et al., 2017; Murray et al., 2018; Squazzoni et al., 2021; Walker et al., n.d.). However, many of these fail to disentangle components of the publication process, focusing only on the outcome of peer-reviewed publications rather than the effect of differential submission rates or desk rejections. These components have fundamentally different attributes—with one being an effect of self-selection and the other a potential indicator of bias.
Most studies focused on this have suggested that women submit fewer papers than men, with little difference in desk rejections (or a disparity in favor of women) (Bendels et al., 2018; Garand & Harman, 2021; Grossman, 2020; Martinsen et al., 2022; Squazzoni et al., 2021). The lower submission rate is particularly pronounced in the most prestigious journals (Bendels et al., 2018). Interviews with women authors explained these lower submission rates with higher selectivity: they argued that they only submit when they are confident the paper would be accepted, not wanting to risk a rejection as they have limited time for conducting research and attending to lengthy reviews (Closa et al., 2020). Women were also less likely than men to indicate that they submit their manuscripts to top journals in their field as their first choice and more likely than men to select a journal based on whether a journal is most likely to accept the manuscript (Djupe et al., 2019). Other perceptions and behavioral differences have also been postulated to account for the differences in manuscript submission behaviors, including risk aversion, perfectionism (Borghans et al., 2009; Closa et al., 2020; Djupe et al., 2019), and assessment of contribution (Lincoln et al., 2012; Wennerås & Wold, 2008). However, few large-scale studies have examined this question from the perspective of the authors. In this context, our paper seeks to address whether the sociodemographic characteristics of authors influence submission rates to elite journals. More specifically, based on a survey of more than 4,700 active authors, we assess differences in gender, academic rank, and discipline in the likelihood of submitting to Science, Nature, and PNAS and the self-reported reasons for not submitting to these journals.
Results
Journal submission behavior
Women were less likely than men to report that they submitted a paper to Science, Nature, or PNAS (48.7% of men and 37.0% of women, see SI Table 7). These differences are statistically significant (see SI Table 8), with men, in general, being more likely to submit to Science, Nature, or PNAS than women while controlling for academic rank (OR=0.61, 95% CI[0.54, 0.70]). This relationship remains significant for each discipline (see Figure 1), with relatively higher percentages of men in each of the disciplines submitting to these journals (ranging from 44.2% to 53.4%), compared to women (ranging from 33.5% to 37.5%). We also investigated the mean number of manuscripts that respondents reported submitting to Science, Nature, or PNAS (see Figure 1 and SI Table 9). The mean was calculated by investigating only respondents who submitted at least one manuscript to Science, Nature, or PNAS. The number of submissions an author could have submitted depends on the discipline (due to the difference in publication behavior between disciplines) and career status (junior, senior, non-academic). In medical sciences (coefficient=-1.46, 95% CI[-2.14, -0.79]) and natural sciences and engineering (coefficient=-0.80, 95% CI[-1.39, -0.22]), women submitted fewer manuscripts than men. No statistically significant difference was observed for the social sciences (see SI Table 10).
Reported acceptance and rejection rate
We examined the difference between women and men regarding the self-reported acceptance and rejection rates for Science, Nature, and PNAS, using linear regression analysis while controlling for academic rank. The result shows no significant difference between women and men in getting their papers accepted (see Figure 1, SI Table 11, and SI Table 12). We also used a mixed-effect model to analyze the role of gender in the chances of getting manuscripts accepted, which yields similar conclusions to those above. We defined the desk rejection rate as the number of manuscripts that did not go out for peer review divided by the number of manuscripts rejected for each survey respondent. The relationship between desk rejection rate and gender were examined using linear regression and a mixed-effect model, again controlling for rank. Both models yield similar results: gender did not significantly impact the possibility of getting a desk rejection for manuscripts submitted to these journals, as illustrated in Figure 1 (as well as SI Table 13 and SI Table 14).
Submission and desk rejections of authors highest cited papers
To understand whether the gender differences observed above also hold for the subset of higher impact research, we investigated respondents’ submission pattern for their most cited papers (not published in Science, Nature, PNAS, but also Cell, Nature Communications, NEJM, and Science Advances; see Methods and Materials for more details). Results show that a substantial proportion of the authors (48.6%) considered other journals than the one in which their paper was published, with no significant gender difference (see SI Table 17 and SI Table 18). However, only a small proportion of authors (9.1%) considered Science, Nature, or PNAS, again with no significant gender difference (see SI Table 19 and SI Table 20). As only a few authors considered publishing in these journals, it is not surprising to see that few authors ever submitted their most cited paper to Science, Nature, or PNAS (228 respondents), again with no significant gender gap (see SI Table 21 and SI Table 22). Similarly, no significant difference was observed in desk rejection rate for their most cited paper between men and women in any of the disciplines when submitting to Science, Nature, and PNAS (see SI Table 23 and SI Table 24). Submission behavior and the chance of getting a desk rejection for higher quality papers thus do not seem to differ significantly between men and women (Figure 2), although these conclusions are based on a very limited number of responses.
Perception of the quality of research
Respondents were asked to rate their perception of the quality of their research (not their papers) in comparison to their peers on a five-point Likert scale ranging from Excellent (5) to Poor (1). Most respondents rated their work as good or excellent (86.7%), regardless of discipline, with very few rating their work as poor or fair (1.0%), and only a few respondents rated it as average (12.4%) (Figure 3 and SI Table 27). When considering responses regardless of discipline, no statistically significant difference was observed between men and women regarding how they rated the quality of their work (see SI Table 28). When disaggregating the respondents by discipline, it was found that compared with the men, women in the natural sciences and engineering are more likely to rank their research quality lower (OR=0.83, 95% CI[0.67, 0.99]). No statistically significant difference was observed in the medical and social sciences.
Reasons for not submitting to elite journals
We also investigated the authors’ rationale for why they did not submit their manuscripts (in general and their most cited) to the selected journals by asking respondents to select from a list of potential reasons. The results of the regression analysis are represented in Figure 4. None of the respondents with a known academic rank indicated that they did not submit to the journals because they were unaware of the journals. The most common reason men and women gave was that their work would fit better in a more specialized journal.
Two of the listed reasons are related to the quality and the novelty of the research. Women in natural sciences and engineering were more likely than men to indicate that “the work was not of high enough quality” as one of the reasons why they did not submit to Science, Nature, or PNAS (OR=1.59, 95% CI[1.22, 2.08]). A similar finding was also found for the most cited paper, although only significant in the medical sciences (OR=0.75, 95% CI[0.60, 0.94]). Regardless of discipline, women were more likely than men to (OR=1.26, 95% CI [1.07, 1.50]) indicate that they consider their “work was not ground-breaking or sufficiently novel” for the journal in question. For their most cited papers, it was also the case for authors publishing in natural sciences and engineering (OR=1.36, 95% CI[1.10, 1.67), as well as all disciplines combined (OR=1.19, 95% CI[1.04, 1.37]) (see SI Table 16 and SI Table 26).
Reasons related to the scope and audience of the journals mostly show non-significant gender differences, with one exception. In general, men were more likely than women to indicate that the “work would fit better in a more specialized journal” for their general submissions (OR=0.72, 95% CI[0.60, 0.85]). When disaggregated by discipline, a significant difference was observed both for the medical sciences (OR=0.74, 95% CI[0.57, 0.95]) and for the natural sciences and engineering (OR=0.72, 95% CI[0.55, 0.95]). However, no statistically significant difference was observed for the most cited papers. Men in the medical sciences were also more likely than women to indicate that the “work would reach a wider audience in another journal” (OR=0.61, 95% CI[0.41, 0.92]). No statistically significant difference was observed for the most cited papers. In contrast to the above results, women submitting in the social sciences were more likely to indicate that the “work fell out of the scope of the journal” (OR=1.84, 95% CI[1.03, 3.28]).
The last two reasons are much less frequent and relate to the wishes of the co-authors and the advice received. In the natural sciences and engineering, women were more likely than men to indicate that their co-authors wished to submit elsewhere (OR=1.70, 95% CI[1.06, 2.74]). This was also the case for their most cited paper (OR=1.57, 95% CI[1.02, 2.40]). Without disaggregating by discipline, women were more likely to indicate that they did not submit their papers (in general and their most cited papers) to Science, Nature, or PNAS because they were advised not to. When disaggregating by discipline, this difference was also observed for respondents in natural sciences and engineering for their papers in general (OR=1.69, 95% CI[1.02, 2.81]) and for medical sciences respondents for their most cited papers (OR=1.75, 95% CI[1.05, 2.93]).
Discussion and Conclusion
Women are particularly underrepresented in journals of the highest scientific impact (Huang et al., 2020), with substantial consequences for their careers. While a large body of research has focused on the outcome and the process of peer review, fewer articles have explicitly focused on gendered submission behavior and the explanations for these differences. In our study of nearly five thousand active authors, we find that women are less likely to report having submitted papers and, when they have, to submit fewer manuscripts, on average, than men. This reinforces much of the literature demonstrating lower submission rates among women (Bendels et al., 2018; Grossman, 2020; Martinsen et al., 2022; Squazzoni et al., 2021). One strategy might be to introduce quotas into peer review; however, this has been shown to have negative effects on women (Leibbrandt et al., 2018). Another strategy might be to optimize around homophily: women editors tend to disproportionately select women reviewers (Helmer et al., 2017), and women reviewers are more favorable to women-authored work (Murray et al., 2018). The positive reinforcement in the process will increase women reviewers and authors, which may influence submission behavior on the broader network.
There is a demonstrated relationship between self-efficacy and publication output (Hemmings & Kay, 2009); however, our results on this were mixed. In the aggregate, no statistically significant difference was observed between men and women in how they rated the quality of their work. A difference was only observed in the natural sciences and engineering, where women ranked their research quality lower. The latter is particularly interesting, given that women tend to outperform in Engineering on several indicators (Ghiasi et al., 2015). The weak signal observed suggests that no particular intervention may be necessary to improve self-efficacy for this population; however, there may be some internalization of value premised on past experiences in peer review. Therefore, addressing larger issues of bias in evaluation may serve to mitigate any gendered differences in self-efficacy.
Regardless of discipline, women were more likely than men to indicate that their “work was not ground-breaking or sufficiently novel” for the listed prestigious journals. Men were more likely than women to indicate that the “work would fit better in a more specialized journal.” Women were more likely to indicate that they did not submit their papers (in general and their most cited papers) to Science, Nature, or PNAS because they were advised not to. These results may reinforce the notion of risk aversion and perfectionism (Closa et al., 2020), suggesting that women have a higher internal standard for what constitutes novel research. However, the more alarming result is that women seem to be deterred from submitting from people within their scholarly network. One interpretation could be that the work is truly not sufficiently novel; this would suggest that men are innately predisposed to higher novelty research designs or that they are trained in this manner. Given no evidence of the former and sufficient evidence of differential mentoring for women (Nolan et al., 2008), it would stand to reason that greater attention should be made to equity in research training. However, the alternative interpretation is that women are not receiving the simple encouragement necessary to seek out higher-impact venues. Research administrators may seek simple interventions to increase this support. Journal editors may also want to consider dedicated outreach to women-led labs. Addressing gender disparities in science requires a multifaceted approach that considers all components of the scientific system. Given that publishing is a cornerstone of this system and central to research evaluation, addressing mechanisms that create disparities in journal submission—be they self-imposed or otherwise—is essential for creating a robust and responsible research ecosystem.
Materials and methods
A survey was conducted to explore the relationship between author gender and manuscript submission, rejection, and acceptance rates for high-impact, multidisciplinary journals in physical sciences, life sciences, and engineering (see S.I. appendix for the questionnaire). This study focuses on the results for Science, Nature, and PNAS. The target population consisted of active authors who published articles from 2008 to 2017 in journals indexed by WoS in medical sciences, natural sciences and engineering, and social sciences (Table S2). The questionnaire asked respondents to report on their manuscript submission experiences. It included a subsection focusing on their most cited paper not published in selected elite journals, i.e., Science, Nature, PNAS (which are analyzed in the main manuscript), as well as Cell, Nature Communications, NEJM, and Science Advances (which are analyzed in the S.I. appendix). Specific questions addressed their experiences submitting their most cited manuscripts to these journals, as well as their experiences submitting to these journals in general. Stata software (Standard Edition 17) was used to conduct the analyses, which consisted of applying ordinal logistic regression, multinomial logistic regression, and multiple logistic regression with a significance level or alpha of 0.05. Although we collected data on genders other than men and women, the independent variable studied is a binary gender variable (i.e., women or men), given the low number of respondents outside these two categories. Respondents were disaggregated by academic rank (i.e., junior, senior, or non-academic, excluding all unknowns) and discipline (i.e., social sciences, medical sciences, and natural sciences and engineering). More details about the regression analysis can be found in the appendix. The sample consisted of 6,002 respondents who participated in the survey, of whom 4,857 finished the questionnaire. An analysis of the attrition failed to identify a common point of departure, suggesting individual variability in dropout rather than failed survey construction. The final number of respondents in this study is 4,805, after the removal of 19 responses in Arts and Humanities and additional responses due to a lack of information for critical variables. Of the final number of respondents, 4,740 (98.6%) have a known academic rank and are included in the regression analysis (more details available in the S.I. appendix SI Table 6).
Data availability
All data needed to evaluate the conclusions in the paper are present in the paper and the Supporting information. Aggregated and de-identified data by gender, discipline, and rank for analyses are available on GitHub (https://github.com/UWMadisonMetaScience/genderPublishing).
Acknowledgements
Nathalie Tufenkji and Vincent Larivière acknowledge funding from the Canada Research Chairs program. The authors would like to thank the survey respondents and the Indiana University Center for Survey Research for their critical contribution to the study. This analysis was approved by McGill University’s Research Ethics Board II, R.E.B. File #: 501-0518 Factors affecting scientific manuscript submission and acceptance rates.
References
- Making “Nature”: The History of a Scientific JournalUniversity of Chicago Press
- Gender disparities in high-quality research revealed by Nature Index journalsPLOS ONE 13https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0189136
- Gender Differences in Risk Aversion and Ambiguity AversionJournal of the European Economic Association 7:649–658https://doi.org/10.1162/JEEA.2009.7.2-3.649
- To Name or Not to Name: The Effect of Changing Author Gender on Peer ReviewBioScience 59:985–989https://doi.org/10.1525/bio.2009.59.11.10
- Mind the (submission) gap: EPSR gender data and female authors publishing perceptionsEuropean Political Science 19:428–442https://doi.org/10.1057/s41304-020-00250-5
- Is there a gender gap in chemical sciences scholarly communication?Chemical Science 11:2277–2301https://doi.org/10.1039/C9SC04090K
- Explaining Gender in the Journals: How Submission Practices Affect Publication Patterns in Political SciencePS: Political Science & Politics 52:71–77https://doi.org/10.1017/S104909651800104X
- Gender differences in authorships are not associated with publication bias in an evolutionary journalPLOS ONE 13https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201725
- Gender differences in peer review outcomes and manuscript impact at six journals of ecology and evolutionEcology and Evolution 9:3599–3619https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.4993
- Journal Desk-Rejection Practices in Political Science: Bringing Data to Bear on What Journals DoPS: Political Science & Politics 54:676–681https://doi.org/10.1017/S1049096521000573
- On the Compliance of Women Engineers with a Gendered Scientific SystemPLOS ONE 10https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0145931
- Is There Gender Bias in JAMA’s Peer Review Process?JAMA 272:139–142https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.1994.03520020065018
- A gender bias in the European Journal of Political Research?European Political Science 19https://doi.org/10.1057/s41304-020-00252-3
- Gender bias in scholarly peer reviewELife 6https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21718
- Lecturer self efficacy: Its related dimensions and the influence of gender and qualificationsIssues in Educational Research 19:243–254
- Historical comparison of gender inequality in scientific careers across countries and disciplinesProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 117:4609–4616https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1914221117
- Intersectional inequalities in scienceProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 119https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2113067119
- Bias in peer reviewJournal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 64:2–17https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.22784
- Gender Quotas, Competitions, and Peer Review: Experimental Evidence on the Backlash Against WomenManagement Science 64:3501–3516https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2017.2772
- The Matilda Effect in science: Awards and prizes in the US, 1990s and 2000sSocial Studies of Science 42:307–320https://doi.org/10.1177/030631271143583
- A gendered pattern? Publishing, submission and reviewing in West European PoliticsEuropean Political Science 21:430–442https://doi.org/10.1057/s41304-021-00356-4
- The Matthew Effect in ScienceScience 159:56–63https://doi.org/10.1126/science.159.3810.56
- Murray, D., Siler, K., Larivière, V., Chan, W. M., Collings, A. M., Raymond, J., & Sugimoto, C. R. (2018). Gender and international diversity improves equity in peer review (p. 400515). bioRxiv. 10.1101/400515Gender and international diversity improves equity in peer review https://doi.org/10.1101/400515
- Nature. (2023). Editorial criteria and processes of Nature. https://www.nature.com/nature/for-authors/editorial-criteria-and-processesEditorial criteria and processes of Nature
- Training and mentoring of chemists: A study of gender disparitySex Roles: A Journal of Research 58:235–250https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9310-5
- PNAS. (2023). Editorial and Journal Policies. PNAS. https://www.pnas.org/author-center/editorial-and-journal-policiesEditorial and Journal Policies. PNAS
- Science publishing: The golden clubNature 502https://doi.org/10.1038/502291a
- Science. (2023). Science Contributors FAQ. https://www.science.org/content/page/science-contributors-faqScience Contributors FAQ
- Peer review and gender bias: A study on 145 scholarly journalsScience Advances 7https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abd0299
- (n.d.). Personal attributes of authors and reviewers, social bias and the outcomes of peer review: A case study [version 2; peer review: 2 approved]F1000Research https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.6012.2
- Peer review in scholarly journals: Perspective of the scholarly community – Results from an international studyInformation Services & Use 28:109–112https://doi.org/10.3233/ISU-2008-0568
- Nepotism and Sexism in Peer-ReviewWomen, Science, and Technology (2nd ed.). Routledge
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2023, Basson et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 778
- downloads
- 18
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.