Abstract
Social animals, including both humans and mice, are highly motivated to engage in social interactions. Short-term social isolation promotes social behavior, but the neural circuits through which it does so remain incompletely understood. Here, we sought to identify neurons that promote social behavior in single-housed female mice, which exhibit increased rates of social investigation, social ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs), and mounting during same-sex interactions that follow a period of short-term (3-day) isolation. We first used immunostaining for the immediate early gene Fos to identify a population of neurons in the preoptic hypothalamus (POA) that increase their activity in single-housed females following same-sex interactions (POAsocial neurons) but not in single-housed females that did not engage in social interactions. TRAP2-mediated chemogenetic silencing of POAsocial neurons in single-housed females significantly attenuates the effects of short-term isolation on social investigation, USV production, and mounting. In contrast, caspase-mediated ablation of POAsocial neurons in single-housed females robustly attenuates mounting but does not decrease social investigation or USV production. Optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons in group-housed females promotes social investigation and USV production but does not recapitulate the effects of short-term isolation on mounting. To understand whether a similar population of POAsocial neurons promotes social behavior in single-housed males, we performed Fos immunostaining in single-housed males following either same-sex or opposite-sex social interactions. These experiments revealed a population of POA neurons that increase Fos expression in single-housed males following opposite-sex, but not same-sex, interactions. Chemogenetic silencing of POAsocial neurons in single-housed males during interactions with females reduces mounting but does not affect social investigation or USV production. These experiments identify a population of hypothalamic neurons that promote social behavior following short-term isolation in a sex- and social context-dependent manner.
Introduction
Humans and other social mammals find social interactions rewarding and are highly motivated to seek out social connections. Consequently, the experience of social isolation is aversive and impacts both our brains and our behaviors. While long-term isolation can lead to the emergence of anti-social behaviors in both humans and rodents (An et al., 2017; Arrigo and Bullock, 2008; Check et al., 1985; Hossain et al., 2020; Killgore et al., 2021; Ma et al., 2011, 2022; Machimbarrena et al., 2019; Matsumoto et al., 2005; Mears and Bales, 2009; Reid et al., 2022; Toth et al., 2011; Valzelli, 1973; Weiss et al., 2004; Wiberg and Grice, 1963; Zelikowsky et al., 2018), short-term isolation typically increases levels of social motivation and promotes social-seeking behaviors (Baumeister and Leary, 1995; Cacioppo et al., 2006; Cacioppo and Cacioppo, 2018; House et al., 1988; Lee et al., 2021; Niesink and van Ree, 1982; Panksepp and Beatty, 1980; Zhao et al., 2021). Alterations in social behavior are characteristic of many neurodevelopmental disorders, including autism spectrum disorder (Chevallier et al., 2012; Clements et al., 2018). How short-term social isolation acts on the brain to promote social behavior remains incompletely understood.
The mesolimbic pathway (i.e., projections from dopaminergic ventral tegmental neurons to the ventral striatum) plays an important role in regulating social motivation and social reward, during courtship as well as during same-sex interactions (Bariselli et al., 2018; Dai et al., 2022; Dölen et al., 2013; Gunaydin et al., 2014; Hung et al., 2017; Love, 2014; Melis et al., 2022; Resendez et al., 2020; Robinson et al., 2011; Solié et al., 2022; Tang et al., 2014; Walum and Young, 2018; Xiao et al., 2017). In line with its role in regulating social behavior, changes in the function of the mesolimbic pathway have been reported following long-term (weeks-long) social isolation and/or early-life social isolation (McWain et al., 2022; Musardo et al., 2022; Tan et al., 2021; Yorgason et al., 2016). The neural circuit changes that mediate the effects of short-term isolation on social behavior in adult animals are comparatively less explored, but here as well, recent studies in both humans and rodents have implicated changes in various populations of midbrain dopamine neurons (Inagaki et al., 2016; Matthews et al., 2016; Tomova et al., 2020). Beyond its effects on mesolimbic circuits, whether social isolation acts on additional neuronal populations to promote social behavior is unknown.
In recent work, we found that short-term (3-day) social isolation exerts robust effects on the social behaviors of C57BL/6J female mice (Zhao et al., 2021). Relative to group-housed females, single-housed females that subsequently engaged in same-sex interactions exhibited increased rates of social investigation, increased rates of USVs, and were also observed to mount female social partners, a behavior never observed in pairs of group-housed females (Zhao et al., 2021). The robust effect of short-term isolation on these three aspects of female social behavior provides a powerful paradigm to identify neurobiological changes that mediate the effects of short-term isolation on social behavior. In the current study, we combined this behavioral paradigm with Fos immunostaining and the TRAP2 activity-dependent labeling approach (Allen et al., 2017; DeNardo et al., 2019) to identify and characterize a population of neurons in the preoptic hypothalamus that increase their activity in single-housed females following same-sex social interactions (i.e., POAsocial neurons). We next asked whether silencing or ablation of POAsocial neurons in single-housed females attenuates female social behaviors, and whether artificial activation of POAsocial neurons in group-housed females promotes social behaviors. Finally, we extended a subset of these experiments to single-housed males engaged in opposite-sex and same-sex interactions, to understand whether short-term isolation acts on the POA to promote social behavior in a manner that depends on either sex or social context. This study identifies novel neurobiological mechanisms through which short-term social isolation acts on the brain to promote social interaction. Our findings also add to an emerging literature indicating that the POA regulates not only sexual behavior but also female social behavior during same-sex interactions.
Results
Neurons in the preoptic hypothalamus increase Fos expression in socially isolated female mice following same-sex social interactions
To identify changes in neuronal activity that may underlie the effects of short-term isolation on female social behavior, we performed immunostaining for the immediate early gene Fos in brain sections collected from group-housed and single-housed (3-days) subject females following 30-minute social encounters in their home cages with a novel, group-housed visitor female (Fig. 1A). In line with our previous behavioral findings (Zhao et al., 2021), we observed that single-housed female residents spent more time investigating visitors (Fig. 1B; t-test, p = 0.001) and in many trials mounted visitors, a behavior that was not observed in group-housed residents (Fig. 1C; 0 of 12 group-housed residents and 11 of 13 single-housed residents mounted visitors; z-test for independent proportions, p < 0.001; see Table S1 for complete statistical details). Female pairs that contained a single-housed resident also produced higher rates of ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) than pairs with a group-housed resident (Fig. 1D; Mann-Whitney U test, p < 0.001). Although either female in a dyad can produce USVs (Warren et al., 2020), the robust effects of short-term isolation on the non-vocal social behaviors of single-housed females suggest that at least some of the elevation in total USVs is driven by increased USV production by the single-housed resident. Given the robust effects of short-term isolation on these three aspects of female social behavior, we focused our analyses on two hypothalamic regions implicated in regulating these behaviors: the preoptic area (POA), which regulates social approach (McHenry et al., 2017), social reward (Hu et al., 2021), mounting (Floody, 1989; Karigo et al., 2021; Wei et al., 2018) and USV production (Chen et al., 2021; Gao et al., 2019; Green et al., 2018; Karigo et al., 2021; Michael et al., 2020); and the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH), which regulates mounting (Hashikawa et al., 2017; Karigo et al., 2021; Lee et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2022). We also examined Fos expression within the caudolateral periaqueductal gray (PAG), based on the well-established role of this region in the control of vocalization in vertebrates and USV production in mice (Chen et al., 2021; Jürgens, 1994; Michael et al., 2020; Tschida et al., 2019). To test whether any observed differences in Fos expression in these three regions were associated with isolation-induced changes in social behavior rather than baseline differences between groups, we also measured Fos expression in the POA, the VMH, and the PAG of group-housed and single-housed females that did not engage in social interaction with novel female visitors (Fig. 1E-F; Fig. S1).
These analyses revealed that baseline levels of Fos expression within the POA and the VMH did not differ between group-housed and single-housed females (Fig. 1F, left and middle, open bars; two-way ANOVA to analyze Fos expression within each brain region, factor 1 = housing status, factor 2 = social interaction, followed by post-hoc Tukey’s HSD tests). Following social interactions with novel female visitors, single-housed females exhibited robust increases in Fos expression within the POA (Fig. 1F, left; p < 0.001) but not within the VMH (Fig. 1F, middle; p > 0.05). In contrast, Fos expression within these two brain areas did not increase significantly in group-housed females that interacted with novel female visitors (Fig. 1F; p > 0.05 for both comparisons). Similar to the POA, baseline Fos expression within the PAG did not differ between group-housed and single-housed females (p > 0.05), and only single-housed females displayed increased PAG Fos expression following social interactions with novel female visitors (Fig. 1F, right; p < 0.001), a finding that further supports the idea that single-housed females increase USV production during same-sex interactions. POA Fos expression was significantly and positively correlated with the total amount of time spent in resident-initiated investigation for both group-housed and single-housed females (Fig. S1C, left; linear regression, p < 0.05), as well as with the total time spent mounting by single-housed resident females (Fig. S1C, middle; p = 0.01). In both group-housed and single-housed female residents, Fos expression tended to correlate positively with total USVs, but these relationships were not significant (Fig. S1C, right; p > 0.05 for both comparisons; see Fig. S1D-E for relationships of VMH Fos and PAG Fos to vocal and non-vocal social behaviors). In summary, POA Fos expression increases selectively in single-housed females following same-sex social interactions, and levels of POA Fos expression are also well related to the production of specific types of social behaviors by both group-housed and single-housed females.
To ask whether the effects of short-term isolation on female social behavior and POA Fos expression are long-lasting, we measured social behaviors of female residents at three timepoints: (1) on day 0, when female subjects were still group-housed; (2) on day 3, after female subjects had been single-housed for 3 days; and (3) on day 17, after half of the subject females had been re-group-housed with their same-sex siblings for two weeks and the other half of the subject females remained single-housed for an additional two weeks (Fig. 1G-I). Brains of re-group-housed and 17-day single-housed subject females were collected after the day 17 social interaction, and Fos expression within the POA was examined (Fig. 1J). Consistent with our earlier findings, rates of social investigation and USV production significantly increased following 3 days of social isolation (Fig. 1G, I; p < 0.05 for day 0 vs. day 3 in both groups for both behaviors). Following re-group-housing, time spent in social investigation and rates of USV production tended to decrease to pre-isolation levels (Fig. 1G, I, top plots; p = 0.08 for day 0 vs. day 17 investigation time and p = 0.06 for day 0 vs. day 17 total USVs in re-group-housed females). In contrast, females that were single-housed for 17 days continued to spend increased time in social investigation (Fig. 1G, bottom plot; p < 0.05 for day 0 vs. day 3 investigation and for day 0 vs. day 17 investigation), and pairs containing 17-day single-housed residents continued to produce elevated rates of USVs (Fig. 1I, bottom plot; p < 0.05 for day 0 vs. day 3 USVs and for day 0 vs. day 17 USVs). Time spent mounting tended to follow the same trends as rates of social investigation and USV production in re-group-housed and 17-day single-housed females (Fig. 1H). Along with the attenuation of female social behaviors following re-group-housing, we also found that POA Fos expression was significantly lower in re-group-housed females relative to 17-day single-housed females (Fig. 1J; t-test, p < 0.001). These findings support the idea that changes in female social behavior following short-term isolation are reversible and are accompanied by decreased POA Fos expression. Hereinafter, we refer to the population of POA neurons that increase Fos expression in single-housed females that have engaged in same-sex interactions as POAsocial neurons, and we next conducted experiments to test whether functional manipulations of POAsocial neuronal activity impact the effects of short-term isolation on female social behavior.
Chemogenetic inhibition of POAsocial neurons attenuates social investigation, mounting, and USV production in single-housed females
If increased activity of POAsocial neurons promotes social behavior in single-housed females, one prediction is that reducing the activity of POAsocial neurons in single-housed females will attenuate the effects of isolation on female social behavior. To test this idea, we employed the TRAP2 activity-dependent labeling strategy to chemogenetically silence POAsocial neurons in single-housed females during social interactions with novel, group-housed female visitors (Fig. 2A). Briefly, the POA of TRAP2 female mice was injected bilaterally with a virus driving the Cre-dependent expression of the inhibitory DREADDs receptor hM4Di. Three weeks later, females were single-housed for 3 days and then given a 30-minute social encounter with a novel, group-housed female visitor in their home cage. Following the social interaction, resident females were given an I.P. injection of 4- hydroxytamoxifen (4-OHT), which drives the transient expression of Cre recombinase in recently active neurons and thereby enables the expression of hM4Di in POAsocial neurons. Subject females remained single-housed for an additional 24 hours and then were re-group-housed with siblings for 2 weeks. Subject females were then single-housed a second time for 3 days and subsequently given a 30-minute same-sex interaction following I.P. injection of either saline (control) or clozapine-n-oxide (CNO) (saline and CNO tests were run 3 days apart, females remained single-housed during these three days, and the order was counterbalanced across experiments).
Comparison of the social behaviors of single-housed females between CNO and saline sessions revealed that chemogenetic silencing of POAsocial neurons significantly reduced resident-initiated investigation (Fig. 2B; N = 12; red points; two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on one factor; p < 0.01). Inhibition of POAsocial neurons also significantly reduced the proportion of females that exhibited mounting (Fig. 2C; McNemar’s test for paired proportions; p = 0.04 for POAsocial CNO vs. saline). Finally, inhibition of POAsocial neurons significantly reduced USV production (Fig. 2D; two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on one factor; p < 0.01). In contrast, CNO treatment did not affect the production of any of these social behaviors in single-housed females with GFP expressed in POAsocial neurons (Fig. 2B-D; N = 14; black points; p > 0.05 for all CNO vs. saline comparisons in the POAsocial-GFP control group). To investigate the specificity of these effects to chemogenetic silencing of POAsocial neurons, we also performed control experiments in which activity-dependent chemogenetic silencing in single-housed females was performed caudal to the POA within the anterior hypothalamus (AH) (Fig. 2B-D; N = 12; brown points) or within the VMH (Fig. 2B-D; N = 5; gray points). No significant effects of CNO treatment on resident-initiated investigation, mounting, or total USVs were observed in these control groups (Fig. 2C-D; p > 0.05 for all). The effect of chemogenetic inhibition of POAsocial neurons to decrease female social behavior also cannot be attributed to an overall decrease in movement (Fig. 2E; paired t-test; p > 0.5 for difference in movement between saline and CNO sessions; see Methods).
We next asked whether the magnitude of the effects of chemogenetic silencing of POAsocial neurons on different female social behaviors was related to the rates at which these behaviors were produced during TRAPing sessions that immediately preceded 4-OHT treatment. To this end, we compared total resident-initiated investigation, total resident-initiated mounting, and total USVs produced during TRAPing sessions to changes in these behaviors for each female following chemogenetic silencing of POAsocial neurons (calculated as (CNO behavior – saline behavior)). These comparisons revealed no significant relationships (Fig. S2A-C; linear regression; p > 0.05 for all). Although we designed our experiments to TRAP and subsequently manipulate the activity of POA neurons that upregulated Fos expression following the production of social behaviors in single-housed females, one possibility is that our strategy inadvertently led to the labeling of POA neurons that upregulate Fos in a manner that reflects the experience of social isolation per se. To test this idea, we performed chemogenetic silencing of POA neurons in females that were single-housed but were not given a social interaction prior to 4-OHT treatment (N = 5 non-social controls; Fig. S2D). In these control single-housed females, CNO treatment had no effect on social investigation, mounting, or USV production during same-sex interactions (Fig. S2E-G; p > 0.05). In summary, we demonstrate that chemogenetic silencing of POAsocial neurons reduces social investigation, mounting, and USV production in single-housed female mice, and that these effects in turn require the production of social behaviors during the TRAPing sessions that precede 4-OHT treatment.
Ablation of POAsocial neurons attenuates mounting in single-housed females
In previous work investigating the role of the POA in regulating rodent social behaviors, studies have reported different effects on behaviors according to whether they employed reversible or irreversible neuronal silencing strategies. Studies that used chemogenetic or optogenetic methods to reversibly silence genetically-defined subsets of POA neurons report decreases in both USV production in males (Chen et al., 2021; Karigo et al., 2021) and in mounting in males and females during interactions with female social partners (Gao et al., 2019; Karigo et al., 2021). In contrast, studies employing caspase-mediated ablation of genetically-defined subsets of POA neurons (Gao et al., 2019; Wei et al., 2018) or electrolytic lesions of the POA (Bean et al., 1981) report decreased mounting but no effects on rates of USV production. To test whether permanent ablation of POAsocial neurons attenuates social behaviors in single-housed females in a manner similar to the effects of chemogenetic inhibition, we used the TRAP2 activity-dependent labeling strategy to express caspase in and to thereby ablate POAsocial neurons (Fig. 3A; see Methods). Vocal and non-vocal social behaviors of resident females were compared pre- and post-ablation, and the same measurements were made in control females expressing GFP in POAsocial neurons.
Similar to the effects of chemogenetically inactivating POAsocial neurons, ablation of POAsocial neurons significantly reduced mounting in single-housed females (Fig. 3C; McNemar’s test for paired proportions; p = 0.03 for proportion of trials with mounting on pre-4-OHT vs. post-4-OHT tests in POAsocial-caspase females; p > 0.05 for pre-4-OHT vs. post-4-OHT in POAsocial-GFP females). In contrast to the effects of chemogenetic inhibition of POAsocial neurons, we found that caspase-mediated ablation of POAsocial neurons did not affect rates of social investigation in single-housed females, although both experimental and control females spent more time investigating visitors in the post-4-OHT session (Fig. 3B; two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on one factor; p > 0.05 for main effect of group, p < 0.01 for main effect of time, p > 0.05 for interaction effect). Ablation of POAsocial neurons also failed to reduce USV production in pairs containing single-housed females (Fig. 3D; two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on one factor; p > 0.05 for pre-4-OHT vs. post-4- OHT total USVs in POAsocial-caspase females). Together with our chemogenetic inhibition data, these results show that both reversible inhibition or irreversible ablation of POAsocial neurons in single-housed female mice reduces mounting, whereas only chemogenetic inhibition of POAsocial neurons attenuates the effects of short-term isolation on female social investigation and USV production.
One possibility is that a difference in the efficacy of the strategies used to irreversibly ablate vs. reversibly inhibit female POAsocial neurons accounts for the difference in their effects on female social behaviors. Indeed, although we used TRAP2 homozygous females in the POAsocial-hM4Di dataset (Fig. 2), the majority of the females used in the POAsocial- caspase dataset were TRAP2 heterozygotes (11 of 15 females were generated by crossing TRAP2 mice with Ai14 mice, and 4 of 15 were TRAP2 homozygotes; absence of tdTomato labeling in TRAP2;Ai14 females was used to estimate viral spread; see Methods). To test this possibility, we directly compared the effects on female social behavior of caspase-mediated ablation of POAsocial neurons in TRAP2 heterozygous females (N = 11 TRAP2;Ai14, a subset of those plotted in Fig. 3B-D) and TRAP2 homozygous females (N = 9, including N = 4 from Fig. 3B-D). These experiments revealed that although ablation of POAsocial neurons in TRAP2 heterozygous females tended to reduce mounting, this effect was non-significant (Fig. S3B; McNemar’s test for paired proportions; p = 0.11). As in the original dataset, ablation of POAsocial neurons in TRAP2 homozygous females significantly reduced mounting (Fig. S3B; p = 0.04). Ablation of POAsocial neurons failed to reduce social investigation or USV production in either genotype, and both of these behaviors were slightly and significantly elevated following 4-OHT treatment in TRAP2 heterozygous females (Fig. S3A,C; p < 0.05 for pre-4-OHT vs. post-4-OHT social investigation and USVs in TRAP2;Ai14 females).
To ensure that ablation of POAsocial neurons in TRAP2 homozygous females was effective in eliminating POA neurons that would normally upregulate Fos following same-sex interactions in single-housed females, we compared counts of Fos-positive POA neurons in a subset of TRAP2 homozygous POAsocial-caspase females to those recorded in control group-housed and single-housed females that either did or did not engage in same-sex interactions (Fig. S3D; control female Fos data are the same as those plotted in Fig. 1D). This analysis revealed that ablation of POAsocial neurons reduced Fos expression in the POA below the levels normally seen in either group-housed or single-housed females following same-sex interactions, down to levels observed in group-housed and single-housed females that were not given social interactions (Fig. S3D; p < 0.05 for POAsocial-caspase vs. group-housed and single-housed social interaction control groups; p > 0.05 for POAsocial-caspase vs. group-housed and single-housed baseline control groups). Moreover, the remaining POA Fos expression in TRAP2 homozygous POAsocial-caspase females was not significantly related to either rates of social investigation or to USV production (Fig. S3E-F; linear regression; p > 0.05 for both; compare to relationships in Fig. S1C). Taken together, these findings show that ablation of POAsocial neurons in TRAP2 homozygous females is more effective at attenuating single-housed female social behaviors than ablation in TRAP2 heterozygotes. However, in spite of a robust reduction in social behavior-driven POA Fos expression, the effects of irreversible ablation of POAsocial neurons on female social behavior differ from those of chemogenetic inhibition.
Optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons elicits USV production
To understand whether artificial activation of POAsocial neurons can recapitulate the effects of short-term isolation on female social behavior, we assessed the effects of optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons on the social behaviors of group-housed females. The TRAP2 strategy was used to express either channelrhodopsin (ChR2) or GFP in POAsocial neurons (Fig. 4A; see Methods), and females were re-group-housed for two weeks before beginning optogenetic activation experiments. The effects of optogenetically activating POAsocial neurons were first assessed for each subject female in a 5-minute solo session, in which the female was tested alone in a behavior chamber while pulses of blue light were delivered unilaterally to the POA (473 nm, 10 mW, 20-50 Hz, 10-20 ms pulses, 5- 10s train durations). The effects of optogenetically activating POAsocial neurons were then assessed for each subject female in a 20-minute social session, in which a novel, group-housed female visitor was added to the behavior chamber. The pair was allowed to interact in the absence of optogenetic stimulation for the first and last 5 minutes of the social session, and pulses of blue light were delivered to the POA of the subject female throughout the middle 10 minutes of the session (Fig. 4A).
When POAsocial-ChR2 females were tested alone, we found that optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons elicited weak-to-moderate USV production in 4 of 8 females, but the comparison of USV rates from pre-laser baseline to the laser stimulation period was not significant at the level of the entire group (Fig. 4B; Mann Whitney U test performed on the difference in USV rates (laser - pre-laser), p = 0.09). In POAsocial-GFP control females, laser stimulation failed to elicit USV production (0 ± 0 USVs elicited in N = 6 POAsocial-GFP controls). Interestingly, we found that when laser stimulation was applied during social sessions, optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons more readily elicited USV production than in solo sessions (Fig. 4C; USVs elicited by blue laser stimulation in 7 of 8 POAsocial- ChR2 females; Mann Whitney U test performed on the difference in USV rates (laser - pre-laser), p = 0.006). Moreover, optogenetic activation elicited higher rates of USVs when applied at times when subject females were in close proximity to visitor females (within 2 mouse body lengths) as compared to times when the females were farther apart (mean increase in USV rates from pre-laser to laser period was 2.96 ± 2.32 USVs/s for “near” stimulations, 1.84 ± 1.75 USVs/s for “far” stimulations; paired t-test performed on the difference in USV rates (laser - pre-laser) for “far” vs. “near” stimulations; p = 0.02). In summary, optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons elicits USV production in group-housed females, and the efficacy of this effect is modulated by social context and proximity to a social partner.
We next considered whether optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons modulates social investigation. To ask whether optogenetic activation could elicit social investigation, we first excluded laser stimulations in which resident-initiated social investigation occurred at any time in a 5-second window prior to laser onset. With these stimulations excluded, we then calculated the proportion of stimulations in which resident-initiated social investigation occurred following laser onset but prior to laser offset. This analysis revealed that POAsocial- ChR2 females were more likely to engage in social investigation of the visitor female following laser stimulation than were POAsocial-GFP females (Fig. 4E-F; p = 0.03 for difference in proportion of laser stimulations followed by social investigation). To ask whether optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons could prolong social investigation bouts, we performed a second analysis in which social investigation bouts that occurred during the middle 10 minutes of the social session were separated into those that temporally overlapped with laser stimulation and those that did not. This analysis revealed that social investigation bouts that overlapped with laser stimulation were significantly longer than those without overlap in POAsocial-ChR2 females but not in POAsocial-GFP females (Fig. 4G; two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on one factor; p = 0.001 for overlapping vs. non-overlapping for POAsocial-ChR2 females; p > 0.05 for overlapping vs. non-overlapping for POAsocial-GFP females). Finally, there was a non-significant trend toward increased overall resident-initiated investigation during the middle 10 minutes of the social session for POAsocial-ChR2 females vs. POAsocial-GFP females (mean total investigation time = 72.1 ± 54.3s for N = 9 POAsocial-ChR2 females vs. 23.3 ± 16.3s for N = 6 POAsocial-ChR2 females; t- test, p = 0.07).
In contrast to the effects on USV production and social investigation, optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons only infrequently elicited mounting (activation elicited n =1 bout of mounting in N = 1 POAsocial-ChR2 female and n = 0 bouts of mounting in the remaining N = 8 POAsocial-ChR2 females). In summary, optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons partially mimics the effects of short-term social isolation on female behavior by promoting USV production and social investigation during same-sex interactions.
Experiments to test whether POA neurons regulate social behaviors during same-sex interactions in group-housed females
Given our findings that POAsocial neurons promote social behaviors in single-housed females during same-sex interactions, we wondered whether a similar population of POA neurons regulates the social behaviors of group-housed females during same-sex interactions. This idea is supported by our earlier finding that Fos expression in the POA of group-housed females is significantly related to rates of resident-initiated social investigation (Fig. S1C, left; p = 0.01) and tends to be related to rates of USV production (Fig. S1C, right; p = 0.05) during same-sex interactions.
To further test this idea, we performed two experiments to manipulate the activity of POA neurons that were TRAPed following same-sex social interactions in group-housed females (Fig. 5A,E). In the first set of experiments, we used the TRAP2 strategy to express hM4Di in POA neurons that upregulated Fos expression following same-sex interactions in group-housed females. Two weeks following 4-OHT treatment, these females were single-housed, and the effect of CNO treatment on female social behavior during same-sex interactions was assessed. We found that chemogenetic inhibition of group-housed-TRAPed POA neurons failed to alter resident-initiated social investigation or total USVs when females were subsequently single-housed and tested with female social partners (Fig. 5B,D; N = 5 group-housed-TRAPed hM4Di females; p > 0.05 for CNO vs. saline sessions for both behaviors). Unfortunately, none of the females in this experimental cohort mounted in the saline or CNO sessions, so we were not able to evaluate the effect on mounting (Fig. 5C). In the second set of experiments, we used the TRAP2 strategy to express ChR2 in POA neurons that upregulate Fos expression following same-sex interactions in group-housed females, and female subjects remained group-housed for subsequent tests. Optogenetic activation of group-housed-TRAPed POA neurons failed to elicit statistically significant USV production (Fig. 5F), did not promote social investigation (Fig. 5G-H), and did not elicit mounting (mounting observed in 0 of 6 trials).
Because group-housed females spend a relatively small amount of time engaged in social investigation, do not exhibit mounting, and produce low rates of USVs relative to single-housed females (Fig. 1B-D), one important caveat to these experiments is that TRAP- based activity-dependent labeling may not work efficaciously in mice that produce low rates of social behaviors. That is to say that although POA neurons may regulate social behaviors in group-housed females, these POA neurons may not strongly upregulate Fos following group-housed social interactions (see Fig. 1F). To test whether the POA regulates the social behaviors of group-housed females using a non-activity-dependent viral strategy, we injected a non-Cre-dependent inhibitory DREAADs virus (AAV-hSyn-hM4Di) into the POA of group-housed B6 female mice (Fig. 5I). Three weeks later, the effects of CNO treatment on social behavior during same-sex interactions were tested. We found no effects of CNO treatment on resident-initiated social investigation in group-housed females (Fig. 5J; p > 0.05), and as expected, none of the group-housed females exhibited mounting in either saline or CNO sessions (Fig. 5K). Notably, we found that although rates of USV production were quite low, chemogenetic inhibition of POA neurons significantly reduced total USVs produced during same-sex interactions between group-housed females (Fig. 5L; paired t- test, p = 0.048). Following these tests, female subjects were single-housed for 3 days and then tested a second time. Consistent with our previous experiments, single-housing dramatically elevated rates of female social investigation, mounting, and USV production (compare saline day panels in Fig. 5J-L vs. Fig. 5M-O). We found that chemogenetic inactivation of POA neurons in single-housed females dramatically reduced all three behaviors (Fig. 5M-O; paired t-tests, p < 0.05 for all behaviors). These additional experiments reinforce our TRAP2-based findings that the POA regulates the social behaviors of single-housed female mice. These experiments also suggest that the POA may regulate the social behaviors of group-housed females, but that any effects may be more challenging to detect given the low rates of social behaviors produced by group-housed females during same-sex interactions.
Characterization of female POAsocial neuron molecular markers and axonal projections
Previous studies have found that USV production can be elicited in female and male mice by artificial activation of VGAT+ POA neurons (Gao et al., 2019), Esr1+ POA neurons (which are predominantly VGAT+) (Chen et al., 2021; Michael et al., 2020), as well as POA neurons that send axonal projections to the caudolateral PAG (which are predominantly VGAT+) (Chen et al., 2021; Michael et al., 2020). To ask to what extent POAsocial neurons overlap with these previously described populations, we first evaluated the neurotransmitter phenotype of POAsocial neurons by performing two-color in situ hybridization for c-fos mRNA and vesicular GABA transporter (VGAT) mRNA and calculating the percentage of Fos+ POA neurons that co-expressed VGAT. This analysis revealed that a majority of POAsocial neurons are GABAergic (Fig. S2A-B; N = 4, 76 ± 8.8%). We next used the TRAP2 activity-dependent labeling strategy to express GFP in POAsocial neurons and found GFP-positive axons within the caudolateral PAG, indicating that at least some POAsocial neurons send axonal projections to the PAG (Fig. S2C-D; see Methods). Finally, we combined retrograde tracing from the caudolateral PAG with Fos immunostaining to quantify the percentage of PAG- projecting POA neurons that increase Fos expression in single-housed females following same-sex interactions. This experiment revealed that around 20% of PAG-projecting POA neurons express Fos in single-housed females following same-sex interactions (Fig. S2E-F; N = 4 females, percentage of tdTomato neurons that are Fos-positive = 18.3 ± 2.9%). These findings suggest that a subset of POAsocial neurons overlap with GABAergic, PAG-projecting POA neurons that have been demonstrated in previous work to promote USVs via disinhibition of excitatory PAG neurons important to USV production, although only ∼20% of PAG-projecting POA neurons upregulate Fos in association with female USV production (Chen et al., 2021; Michael et al., 2020).
POA neurons increase their activity in single-housed male mice following opposite-sex but not same-sex social interactions
Given our findings that POAsocial neurons contribute to isolation-induced changes in the social behaviors of female mice, we next wondered whether a similar population of POA neurons contributes to isolation-induced changes in social behavior in male mice. To address this question, we measured the vocal and non-vocal social behaviors of sexually naïve males, which were either group-housed with same-sex siblings or single-housed for three days and then given a 30-minute social interaction with a novel, group-housed visitor. To consider the effects of isolation on male social behavior in different social contexts, males were given either a social encounter with a same-sex visitor (MM context) or with an opposite-sex visitor (MF context). Following these social interaction tests, we collected the brains of the subject males and performed immunostaining to measure Fos expression within the POA. With respect to resident-initiated investigation, we found significant main effects of both housing and social context: single-housed males spent more time investigating visitors than group-housed males, and males spent more time investigating female visitors than male visitors (Fig. 5A; two-way ANOVA, p = 0.02 for main effect of housing; p < 0.001 for main effect of social context; p > 0.05 for interaction effect). With respect to mounting, we found that single-housed males were more likely than group-housed males to mount female visitors, but the proportion of trials with mounting did not differ for single-housed males vs. group-housed males during male-male interactions (Fig. 5B; z-tests for independent proportions; p = 0.046 for group-housed vs. single-housed with female visitor, p > 0.05 for group-housed vs. single-housed with male visitor). Similarly, there was a context-dependent effect of social isolation on male USV production, whereby only single-housed males that interacted with female visitors exhibited increased USV production relative to group-housed males (Fig. 5C; two-way ANOVA with post-hoc Tukey’s HSD tests; p < 0.001 for total USVs in single-housed MF vs. group-housed MF trials; p > 0.05 for total USVs in single-housed MM vs. group-housed MM trials). The finding that short-term isolation exerts larger effects on male social behavior during subsequent opposite-sex interactions relative to same-sex interactions is consistent with prior work (Zhao et al., 2021). When we examined POA Fos expression in these four groups of males, we found that POA Fos was significantly elevated in single-housed males following interactions with females relative to the other three groups (Fig. 5D; two-way ANOVA, Tukey’s post-hoc HSD tests; p < 0.05 for difference in POA Fos between single-housed MF and all other groups). In summary, the effects of short-term isolation on male social behavior are context-dependent, and increased Fos expression within the POA is seen in single-housed males following interactions with females, a context marked by increased male social investigation, mounting, and USV production.
To test whether neural activity in male POAsocial neurons contributes to isolation-induced changes in male social behavior, we used the TRAP2 strategy to chemogenetically silence POAsocial neurons in single-housed males during social interactions with novel, group-housed females (see Methods). The vocal and non-vocal behaviors of single-housed subject males were measured and compared during 30-minute social interactions following I.P. injection of either saline or CNO. Control males were treated identically but were injected with a virus to drive expression of GFP in POAsocial neurons. As in females, chemogenetic inhibition of male POAsocial neurons significantly reduced the proportion of trials with mounting (Fig. 5G; z-test for difference between proportions; p = 0.04 for proportion of trials with mounting on CNO vs. saline days in POAsocial-hM4Di males; p > 0.05 for proportion of trials with mounting on CNO vs. saline days in POAsocial-GFP males). In contrast to our findings in females, chemogenetic inhibition of male POAsocial neurons did not alter rates of resident-initiated social investigation (Fig. 5F; two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on one factor; p < 0.001 for main effect of group; p > 0.05 for main effect of drug and for interaction effect) and also did not affect total USVs (Fig. 5H; two-way ANOVA with repeated measures on one factor; p > 0.05 for main effects and interaction effect). Although we are uncertain of the reason for the unexpected difference in social investigation times between POAsocial-hM4Di and POAsocial-GFP males, we note that these cohorts of mice were tested at different times and thus interacted with different groups of female visitors, which may have in turn contributed to different rates of social investigation between experimental and control males.
Given previous work implicating the POA in the regulation of male mounting (Floody, 1989; Karigo et al., 2021; Wei et al., 2018) as well as male USV production (Chen et al., 2021; Gao et al., 2019; Green et al., 2018; Karigo et al., 2021; Michael et al., 2020), we were surprised that chemogenetic silencing of POAsocial neurons in males attenuated mounting but not USV production. To begin to understand this result, we considered rates of social behaviors produced by POAsocial-hM4Di males during the TRAPing sessions that preceded 4-OHT treatment and compared these to the TRAPing session behaviors produced by POAsocial-hM4Di females. Interestingly, we found that POAsocial-hM4Di males spent significantly more time mounting female visitors during TRAPing sessions than POAsocial- hM4Di females (Fig. S5B; Mann Whitney U test; p = 0.004). In comparison to females, POAsocial-hM4Di males spent less time engaged in social investigation of the female visitor during TRAPing sessions (Fig. S5A; t-test; p = 0.02) and also tended to produce lower rates of USVs (Fig. S5C; t-test, p = 0.054). Based on these sex differences in TRAPing session social behavior, one possibility is that the bias of male TRAPing session behavior toward mounting also resulted in a bias toward TRAP2-based labeling (and subsequent chemogenetic inhibition) of POA mounting-related neurons. We note also that because all animals in the study received the same total amount of 4-OHT (150 µL of 10 mg/mL 4-OHT) and because males typically weigh more than females, the effectively smaller dosage of 4- OHT delivered to males could have reduced TRAPing efficacy relative to females and could have contributed to sex differences in the observed effects on behavior of chemogenetically silencing POAsocial neurons. In summary, we find that chemogenetic silencing of male POAsocial neurons reduces mounting during subsequent social interactions with females but does not reduce social investigation or USV production, a pattern of results that differs from the effects on single-housed female social behavior of chemogenetically silencing female POAsocial neurons.
Discussion
In the current study, we identify and characterize a population of preoptic hypothalamic neurons that contribute to the effects of short-term social isolation on the social behaviors of mice. These POAsocial neurons exhibit increased Fos expression in single-housed female mice following same-sex social interactions, and this increase in Fos expression scales positively with the time females spend investigating and mounting female visitors and tends also to scale with rates of USVs. Chemogenetic silencing of POAsocial neurons attenuates the effects of social isolation on female social behavior, significantly reducing social investigation, mounting, and USV production. In contrast, irreversible ablation of POAsocial neurons significantly reduces mounting in single-housed females but does not reduce rates of social investigation or USVs. Optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons partially recapitulates the effects of short-term isolation on female behavior and promotes social investigation and USV production in female mice. Finally, we extended our experiments to male mice to understand whether similar POA neurons may mediate changes in male social behavior following short-term isolation. We find that short-term isolation exerts more robust effects on male behavior during subsequent interactions with females than during subsequent interactions with males, and increased POA Fos expression is seen in single-housed males following social interactions with females but not following interactions with males. Interestingly, chemogenetic silencing of these POAsocial neurons reduces mounting but has no effect on social investigation and USV production, in contrast to the effects of chemogenetically silencing POAsocial neurons in females. Together, these experiments identify a population of preoptic hypothalamic neurons that promote social behaviors in single-housed mice in a manner that depends on sex and social context.
An extensive body of past work has implicated the POA in the regulation of male sexual behavior, including in the regulation of male courtship vocalizations in both rodents and birds (Alger and Riters, 2006; Bean et al., 1981; Gao et al., 2019; Merari and Ginton, 1975; Riters, 2012; Riters et al., 2000; Riters and Ball, 1999; Wei et al., 2018). Past work has also shown that activation of genetically-defined subsets of POA can elicit the production of USVs in both male and female mice (Chen et al., 2021; Gao et al., 2019; Karigo et al., 2021; Michael et al., 2020). However, whether the POA regulates natural USV production in female mice remained unclear. In the current study, we demonstrate that reversible silencing of POA neurons that increase their activity during same-sex interactions decreases female USV production, indicating that the POA regulates the production of USVs by single-housed females engaged in same-sex interactions. Using a non-activity-dependent viral strategy, we also provide evidence that POA neurons regulate USV production in group-housed females. Whether these same neurons regulate female USV production in other behavioral contexts, including during interactions with male partners, remains an important open question. In addition to attenuating USV production, we found that silencing of POAsocial neurons in single-housed females significantly reduced social investigation and mounting of same-sex partners. Our retrograde and anterograde tracing experiments demonstrate that at least a subset of POAsocial neurons project to the caudolateral PAG where neurons important for USV production reside, and an attractive possibility is that POAsocial neurons promote USV production via disinhibition of PAG-USV neurons as previously demonstrated for genetically-defined subsets of POA neurons (Chen et al., 2021; Michael et al., 2020; Tschida et al., 2019). Future experiments will be required to determine whether PAG-projecting POAsocial neurons also regulate social investigation and mounting, or alternatively, whether distinct molecularly-defined or projection-defined subsets of POAsocial neurons differentially regulate these different aspects of female social behavior. A recent study found that optogenetic activation of Tacr1+ POA neurons elicits both mounting and USV production in male mice and that optogenetic activation of the axon terminals of these neurons within the PAG also promotes mounting, suggesting that some POA neurons may regulate the production of multiple social behaviors. However, another study found that distinct subsets of male Esr1+ POA neurons exhibit selective tuning in their neuronal activity to sniffing vs. mounting during interactions with females (Yang et al., 2023), although it remains to be tested whether these neuronal subsets functionally regulate distinct social behaviors during male-female (or female-female) interactions. The latter organization would be reminiscent of how projection-defined subsets of galanin-expressing POA neurons regulate different aspects of parental behavior (Kohl et al., 2018).
Although reversible inhibition of POAsocial neurons reduced both social investigation and USV production in single-housed females, permanent caspase-mediated ablation of these neurons significantly reduced mounting but had no effects on social investigation or USV production. These differences are largely consistent with prior studies that reported effects on both mounting and USV production following reversible manipulations of POA activity and effects on mounting but not on USV production following irreversible manipulations of POA activity (Bean et al., 1981; Chen et al., 2021; Gao et al., 2019; Karigo et al., 2021; Wei et al., 2018). One possibility is that POAsocial neurons do not directly regulate USV production and social investigation but rather that reversible silencing of these neurons causes off-target disruptions of neural activity in interconnected brain regions that in turn directly regulate USV production. Such a relationship was demonstrated for motor cortex, whereby reversible silencing of motor cortex disrupted performance of a learned forelimb reaching task in rats, while permanent lesions of motor cortex had no effect on task performance after learning (Otchy et al., 2015). However, our finding that optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons elicits USV production, along with past work demonstrating that POA activation elicits USV production (Chen et al., 2021; Gao et al., 2019; Karigo et al., 2021; Michael et al., 2020) supports the idea that the POA directly regulates USV production. Similarly, our finding that optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons promotes social investigation supports the interpretation that these neurons directly regulate this behavior. An alternative explanation for the apparently contradictory effects of chemogenetic silencing vs. ablation of POAsocial neurons is that while the POA plays an obligatory role in mounting behavior (at least in certain contexts, see below), compensatory changes in additional, non-POA circuits that promote USV production and social investigation are sufficient to compensate for the permanent loss of POAsocial neurons, leaving these behaviors unperturbed following POAsocial neuronal ablation. Our finding that ablation of POAsocial neurons dramatically reduces POA Fos expression following female-female interactions, and that the remaining Fos expression is uncorrelated with rates of USV production and social investigation, support the latter idea. The identification of forebrain-to-midbrain circuits that regulate USV production in both females and males remains an important future goal.
Our finding that POAsocial neurons regulate female-female mounting extends recent work examining the role of hypothalamic regions in regulating male mounting behavior in different social contexts (Karigo et al., 2021). Although the POA plays a well-established role in the regulation of mounting behavior by male rodents during interactions with females, interactions which are typically marked by high rates of USV production and are considered affiliative, the authors found that the VMH regulates male mounting behavior during interactions with other males, which is typically not accompanied by USV production and often precedes fighting. Taken together with the findings of the current study, we conclude that the POA regulates mounting behavior in both male and female mice that is directed toward female social partners and is accompanied by USV production. The question of whether the social behaviors exhibited by single-housed female mice during same-sex interactions are indeed affiliative and how these behaviors shape future interactions with female social partners are important questions that remain to be addressed. Although chemogenetic silencing and ablation of POAsocial neurons reduced female mounting, we were not able to reliably elicit mounting behavior through optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons. We note that previous studies that elicited mounting in male mice via optogenetic activation of Esr1+ POA neurons, Tacr1+ POA neurons, or POA neurons that co-express Esr1 and VGAT used longer laser stimulation trains (15-30s) than we employed in the current study (Bayless et al., 2023; Karigo et al., 2021; Wei et al., 2018). It is possible that longer periods of optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons would be more effective in eliciting mounting in group-housed females, and another possibility is that POAsocial neurons overlap somewhat but not perfectly with the genetically-defined groups of POA neurons manipulated in those prior studies. Regardless, our finding that optogenetic activation of POAsocial neurons promotes USV production and social investigation efficaciously relative to mounting indicates that effects on these behaviors are not secondary to effects on female mounting.
To understand whether a similar population of POA neurons regulates changes in social behavior following short-term isolation in males, we extended our experiments to single-housed males that subsequently interacted with either a male social partner or with a female social partner. Consistent with our prior work (Zhao et al., 2021), we found that short-term isolation exerts more robust effects on male behavior during subsequent interactions with females than during interactions with males. Although single-housed males exhibited increased rates of social investigation while interacting with both males and females relative to group-housed males, single-housed males that interacted with females exhibited increases in all measured social behaviors (social investigation, USV production, and mounting), a pattern of behavioral changes that is similar to what we observed in single-housed females. We also observed that a population of POA neurons increased Fos expression in single-housed males following interactions with female visitors but not following interactions with male visitors. Unexpectedly, we found that the effects of chemogenetically inhibiting POAsocial neurons in males differed from those in females. Namely, while reversible inhibition of POAsocial neurons reduced mounting during interactions between single-housed subject males and female visitors, there were no effects on rates of social investigation and USV production. These findings differ also from recent work showing that chemogenetic inhibition (Chen et al., 2021) or optogenetic inhibition (Karigo et al., 2021) of Esr1+ POA neurons reduces USV production in male mice. Although the factors that account for this difference in results are unclear, one possibility is that our TRAP2-based viral labeling in males is biased toward POA neurons that regulate mounting as compared to POA neurons that regulate USV production, although as stated above, more work is required to understand whether these different social behaviors are regulated by distinct or overlapping subsets of POA neurons. Future experiments can also explore to what extent female and male POAsocial neurons represent molecularly, anatomically, and functionally similar or dissimilar neuronal populations.
The current study adds to an emerging body of literature implicating the POA in the regulation of social behavior in both females and males, including in non-sexual social contexts (Fukumitsu et al., 2022; Hu et al., 2021; Liu et al., 2023; McHenry et al., 2017; Wu et al., 2021). Our findings also complement recent work that has described a role for the POA in regulating behavioral responses to social isolation in female mice. A recent study using BALB/c mice found that reunion with same-sex cagemates following short-term “somatic isolation” (i.e., separation from cagemates via a partition that permits visual, auditory, and olfactory signaling but prevents physical contact) increases the activity of calcitonin-receptor (Calcr) expressing POA neurons (Fukumitsu et al., 2023). Knockdown of Calcr expression in these neurons reduced social-seeking behaviors directed at the cage partition exhibited by single-housed females, and chemogenetic activation of these neurons increased partition-biting behavior in single-housed females. Another recent study using female FVB/NJ mice applied a TRAP2-based intersectional approach to identify POA neurons that increase their activity following reunion with same-sex cagemates (MPNreunion neurons) (Liu et al., 2023). Optogenetic activation of these neurons did not alter social investigation in group-housed females, whereas activation of these neurons during reunion with cagemates following short-term social isolation decreased social investigation. However, optogenetic inhibition of MPNreunion during reunion did not alter rates of social investigation in single-housed females. Although we did not test the effects of activating POAsocial neurons in single-housed females in the current study, the difference in the effects of inhibiting POAsocial neurons on the behavior of single-housed females (reduced rates of social investigation, USV production, and mounting) relative to the effects of optogenetic inhibition of MPNreunion neurons in Liu et al. (no effect on social investigation during reunion) suggest that these represent distinct populations of POA neurons. We note that in the current study, single-housed female subjects remained single-housed for 24 hours following social interaction TRAPing sessions, a design intended to maximize viral labeling of POA neurons that promote increased female social behaviors and that would likely in turn minimize viral labeling of POA neurons that promote social satiety. These studies highlight a complex role for the POA in regulating multiple aspects of changes in social behavior following short-term social isolation. Although some studies of social isolation have avoided the use of C57BL/6J female mice, the robust triad of changes in social behavior exhibited by these females following short-term isolation affords a powerful opportunity to continue investigating neural circuit mechanisms through which short-term social isolation promotes social behaviors, as well as to investigate how hypothalamic circuits regulate the coordinated production of suites of social behaviors during female-female social interactions.
Materials and methods
Key Resources
Lead contact
Further information and requests for resources should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, Katherine Tschida (kat227@cornell.edu).
Experimental models and subject details
Animal statement
All experiments and procedures were conducted according to protocols approved by the Cornell University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol #2020-001).
Animals
TRAP2 (Jackson Laboratories, 030323), Ai14 (Jackson Laboratories, 007914), TRAP2;Ai14, and C57Bl/6J (Jackson Laboratories, 000664) mice were at least 8 weeks old at the time of the experiments or surgeries. TRAP2;Ai14 mice were generated by crossing TRAP2 with Ai14. All mice were kept on a 12:12 reversed light/dark cycle, were housed in ventilated micro-isolator cages in a controlled environment with regulated temperature and humidity and were provided with unrestricted access to food and water. A running wheel (Innovive) was present in all home cages from the time of weaning and was subsequently removed immediately before initiating the social interaction test. Mouse cages were cleaned weekly, and experiments were never conducted on cage change days.
TRAP2 female and male subjects were used in all experiments, with the exception of N = 11 TRAP2;Ai14 females subjects used in the neuronal ablation experiments (Fig. 3, S3), N = 4 Ai14 females used in anatomical experiments to examine overlap between PAG-projecting POA neurons and POA neurons that upregulate Fos following same-sex interactions in single-housed females (Fig. S4), and N = 7 B6 females used for non-Cre-dependent chemogenetic silencing of POA (Fig. 5I-O).
Methods details
Social isolation and social interaction tests
Female and male subject mice were either group-housed with same-sex siblings or separated from their cage mates and individually housed in clean cages for three days prior to behavioral tests. In the case of group-housed subject mice, siblings were temporarily removed from the home cage for the duration of the test. The subject animal’s home cage was then placed in a sound-attenuating recording chamber (Med Associates) equipped with an ultrasonic microphone (Avisoft), an infrared light source (Tendelux), and a webcam (Logitech, with the infrared filter removed to enable video recording under infrared lighting conditions). A novel, group-housed visitor mouse (female or male mouse on a C57BL/6 background) was placed in the home cage of the subject mouse, and vocal and non-vocal behaviors were recorded for 30 minutes. Visitor mice were used across multiple experiments (< 6 in total), including in interactions with both group-housed and single-housed subject mice. Visitor females used in male-female interactions were never used for female-female experiments, but a subset of visitors used in female-female interactions were subsequently used in male-female experiments.
A separate cohort of female mice was used to investigate the effects on social behavior of re-group-housing following a period of social isolation. For this cohort of mice, social interaction tests with novel, group-housed female visitors were conducted at three timepoints: (1) on day 0, when subject females were still group-housed; (2) On day 3, after being single-housed for 3 days; (3) on day 17, after a randomly selected subset of subject females were re-group-housed with their siblings for two weeks, and the remaining female subjects remained single-housed for two weeks.
USV recording and detection
USVs were recorded with an ultrasonic microphone (Avisoft, CMPA/CM16), amplified (Presonus TubePreV2), and digitized at 250 kHz (Avisoft UltrasoundGate 166H or CED Power 1401). USVs were detected with custom MATLAB codes (Tschida et al., 2019) using the following parameters (mean frequency > 45 kHz; spectral purity > 0.3; spectral discontinuity < 1.00; minimum USV duration = 5 ms; minimum inter-syllable interval = 30 ms).
Analyses of non-vocal social behaviors
Trained observers used BORIS software (v.8.13; Friard and Gamba, 2016) to score the following non-vocal behaviors: resident-initiated social investigation and resident-initiated mounting. Social investigation included sniffing and following. Resident-initiated mounting of the visitor typically occurred following a period of resident-initiated social investigation, with the resident mouse positioning its forelimbs on top of the body of the visitor, sometimes with pelvic thrusts and sometimes without. Neither visitor-initiated mounting nor fighting were observed in our dataset.
In some trials, total movement was estimated using a custom MATLAB code that allows the user to mark the position of a mouse in every 30th frame (i.e., once per second). Total movement was then calculated as the sum of changes in position across pairs of marked frames.
Fos immunohistochemistry
Two hours following the start of the social interaction test, mice were deeply anesthetized using isoflurane and then transcardially perfused with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS, pH 7.4), followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA; Sigma-Aldrich, in 0.1 M PBS, pH 7.4). Brains were subsequently dissected and post-fixed in 4% PFA for 24 hours at 4°C, followed by immersion in 30% sucrose solution in PBS for 48 hours at 4°C. Afterward, brains were embedded in frozen section embedding medium (Surgipath, VWR), flash frozen in a dry ice-ethanol (100%) bath, and then stored at –80°C until sectioning. Sections were cut on a cryostat (Leica CM1950) to a thickness of 80 μm, washed in PBS (3 x 5 mins at RT), permeabilized for 2-3 hours in PBS containing 1% Triton X-100 (PBST), and then blocked in 0.3% PBST containing 10% Blocking One (Nacalai USA) for 1 hour at RT on a shaker. Sections were then incubated for 24 hours at 4°C with primary antibody in blocking solution (1:1000 rabbit-anti-Fos, Cell Signaling Technologies, 2250S), washed 3 x 30 minutes in 0.3% PBST, then incubated for 24 hours at 4°C with secondary antibody in blocking solution (1:1000, Alexa Fluor 488 goat-anti-rabbit, Invitrogen, plus 1:500 NeuroTrace, Invitrogen) Finally, sections were washed for 2 x 10 minutes in 0.3% PBST, followed by washing for 2 x 10 minutes in PBS. After mounting on slides, sections were dried and coverslipped with Fluromount G (Southern Biotech). Slides were imaged with a 10x objective on a Zeiss 900 laser scanning confocal microscope, and Fos-positive neurons within regions of interest were counted manually by trained observers.
Floating section two-color in situ hybridization
In situ hybridization was conducted using hybridization chain reaction (HCR v3.0, Molecular Instruments). Ten minutes after the completion of the 30-minute social interaction tests, mice underwent transcardial perfusion with RNase-free PBS (DEPC-treated), followed by 4% PFA. Dissected brain samples were post-fixed overnight in 4% PFA at 4°C, cryoprotected in a 30% sucrose solution in DEPC-PBS at 4°C for 48 hours, flash frozen in section embedding medium, and stored at –80°C until sectioning. 40 μm-thick coronal floating sections were collected into sterile 24-well plates in DEPC-PBS. These sections were briefly fixed once again for 5 minutes in 4% PFA and subsequently immersed in 70% EtOH in DEPC-PBS overnight. Sections were then rinsed in DEPC-PBS, incubated for 45 minutes in 5% SDS in DEPC-PBS, followed by a series of rinses and incubations: 2x SSCT, pre-incubation in HCR hybridization buffer at 37°C, and incubation in HCR hybridization buffer containing RNA probes (VGAT and Fos) overnight at 37°C. Sections were then rinsed 4 x 15 minutes at 37°C in HCR probe wash buffer, rinsed in 2X SSCT, pre-incubated in HCR amplification buffer, and then incubated in HCR amplification buffer containing HCR amplifiers at RT for approximately 48 hours. On the final day, sections were rinsed in 2x SSCT, counterstained with DAPI (Thermo Fisher, 1:5000), rinsed again with 2x SSCT, mounted on slides, and coverslipped with Fluoromount-G (Southern Biotech). After drying, slides were imaged with a 10x or 20x objective on a Zeiss 900 laser scanning microscope. Neurons were scored from three sections of tissue from the POA from each mouse, and the absence of presence of staining for different probes was quantified manually by trained scorers.
Viruses
The following viruses and injection volumes were used: AAV2/1-hSyn-FLEX-hM4Di-mCherry (Addgene #44262, 200 nL), AAV2/1-CAG-FLEX-EGFP-WPRE (Addgene #51502, 200 nL), AAV2/5-Ef1alpha-FLEX-taCasp3-TEVp (Addgene #45580, 200 nL), AAV2/1-Ef1alpha-hChR2(h134R)-EYFP-WPRE-HGHpA (Addgene #20298, 200 nL), AAVrg-pgk-Cre (Addgene #24593, 200 nL), and AAV2/5-hSyn-hM4Di-mCherry (Addgene #50475, 200 nL). The final injection coordinates were as follows: POA, AP = −0.1 mm, ML = 0.6 mm, DV = 5.1 mm; AH, AP = −0.7 mm, ML = 0.6 mm, DV = 5.1 mm; VMH, AP = −1.5 mm, ML = 0.7 mm, DV = 5.4 mm; PAG, AP = −4.7 mm, ML = 0.6 mm, DV = 1.75 mm. Viruses were pressure-injected using a pulled glass pipettes mounted in a programmable nanoliter injector (Nanoject III, Drummond) at a rate of 15 nL every 60 s.
Stereotaxic Surgery
Mice were anesthetized using isoflurane (2.5% for induction, then 1.5 - 2.5% for maintenance) and then securely positioned in a stereotaxic apparatus (Angle Two, Leica). A midline incision in the scalp was made to expose the skull, and small craniotomies were created dorsal to each injection site using a surgical drill. Viral injection pipettes were left in place for a minimum of 10 minutes before and after viral injections to minimize backflow upon pipette withdrawal from the brain. Surgical sutures (LOOK 774B, Fisher Scientific) and tissue adhesive (3M) were used to close the incision.
For optogenetic activation experiments, an optogenetic ferrule (RWD Fiber Optic Cannula, Ø1.25 mm Ceramic Ferrule, 200 µm Core, 0.22 NA, L = 7 mm) was implanted approximately 250 µm above the viral injection site immediately following the viral injection and was secured in place with Metabond (Parkell).
TRAP activity-dependent labeling
Solutions of 4-hydroxytamoxifen (4-OHT, HelloBio, HB6040) were prepared by dissolving 4- OHT powder at 20 mg/mL in ethanol by shaking at 37°C, and aliquots (75 uL) were then stored at −20°C. Before use, 4-OHT was redissolved in ethanol by shaking at 37°C and filtered corn oil was added (150 uL). Ethanol was then evaporated by vacuum under centrifugation to give a final concentration of 10 mg/mL, and the 4-OHT solution was used on the same day it was prepared.
To express viral transgenes in recently active neurons, we used the Targeted Recombination in Active Populations (TRAP2) strategy. Three weeks following viral injection, TRAP2 and TRAP2;Ai14 mice were single-housed for 3 days and then given 30- minute social encounters (as described above). Following the social encounter, subject mice received I.P. injections of 4-OHT (150 uL of 10 mg/mL 4-OHT in filtered corn oil) to enable expression of viral transgenes in recently active neurons. To minimize neural activity triggered by stimuli outside of the social interaction test, all subject animals were individually housed for an additional 24 hours following 4-OHT treatment before being re-group-housed with their same-sex siblings. In some control experiments, mice remained group-housed prior to the 30-minute social encounter and 4-OHT treatment (Fig. 5A-H) or were single-housed but were not given a social encounter prior to 4-OHT treatment (Fig. S2D-G).
Chemogenetic inhibition
To reversibly reduce neuronal activity, TRAP2 female mice received bilateral injections of an Cre-dependent inhibitory DREADDs virus into the hypothalamus (AAV2/1-hSyn-FLEX- hM4Di-mCherry; injected into the POA, AH, or VMH) as described above. TRAP2 male mice received the same viral injections into the POA only. Three weeks later, mice were single-housed for 3 days and then were subsequently given a 30-minute social encounter in their home cage with a novel, group-housed female visitor. Subject mice then received an I.P. injection of 4-OHT to enable expression of hM4Di in activity-defined populations of hypothalamic neurons. Following the TRAPing session, subjects remained single-housed for an additional 24 hours. Subjects were then re-group-housed with same-sex siblings for two weeks and were single-housed a second time prior to behavioral testing. On the first day of testing, subject mice received an I.P. injection of either sterile saline (as a control) or clozapine-n-oxide (CNO, 4 mg/kg, Hello Bio HB6149; to inhibit neurons expressing hM4Di) 30 minutes prior to a social interaction test. Three days later, mice that previously were treated with saline received an I.P. injection of CNO, and mice that were previously treated with CNO received an I.P. injection of saline, 30 minutes prior to another social interaction. Rates of USV production and non-vocal social behaviors were compared between saline and CNO days within animals to assess the effects of neuronal inhibition on social behaviors. Control mice received unilateral injections into the POA of a Cre-dependent AAV driving the expression of GFP (AAV2/1-CAG-FLEX-EGFP) and were otherwise treated identically to experimental animals.
Neuronal ablation
To permanently ablate neurons, TRAP2;Ai14 female mice received bilateral injections of an AAV2/5-ef1alpha-FLEX-taCasp3-TEVp virus into the POA. Following a three-week recovery period, these animals were individually housed for three days and subsequently given a 30- minute social encounter in their home cage with a novel, group-housed female visitor. Subject mice then received an I.P. injection of 4-OHT to enable expression of caspase in activity-defined POA neurons. Two weeks later, females were single-housed for three days and then given a second 30-minute social interaction test. Social behaviors of subject females were compared between the pre-ablation and post-ablation interaction tests to assess the effects of neuronal ablation.
Optogenetic activation
Female TRAP2 mice received unilateral injections into the POA of AAV-ef1α-FLEX-ChR2 (experimental) or AAV-CAG-FLEX-GFP (control). In the same surgery, an optogenetic ferrule was implanted approximately 250 µm above the viral injection site. Three weeks later, females were single-housed for 3 days and then given a 30-minute social interaction with a novel group-housed, female visitor. Subject females then received an I.P. injection of 4- OHT. Two weeks later, females were first placed alone in a clean testing chamber for a 5- minute habituation period after connecting the laser patch cable to the female’s optogenetic ferrule. Optogenetic activation sessions consisted of a 5-minute period in which optogenetic activation was performed in solo females, followed by a 20-minute period in which activation was performed as subject females interacted with a novel, group-housed female visitor. The social session was further divided into three phases: 5 minutes without optogenetic activation, 10 minutes with optogenetic activation, and 5 minutes without optogenetic activation. During the middle 10 minutes of the social session, some laser stimuli were delivered at times when the two females were near to one another (inter-animal distance < ∼2 mouse body lengths), and other stimuli were delivered at times when the females were not in close contact. POAsocial neurons were optogenetically activated with illumination from a 473 nM laser (10 mW) at 20-50 Hz (10-20 ms pulses, trains lasted 5-10s) Laser stimuli were driven by computer-controlled voltage pulses (Spike 2 version 10.8, CED).
Anatomical tracing
Female TRAP2 mice used as GFP controls in the chemogenetic inhibition experiments were subsequently used for anterograde mapping of the axonal projections of POAsocial neurons. Three weeks following unilateral injection into the POA of a Cre-dependent AAV driving the expression of GFP (AAV2/1-CAG-FLEX-EGFP), females were single-housed for 3 days and subsequently given a 30-minute social interaction test. Subject mice then received an I.P. injection of 4-OHT. Six weeks later, females were perfused, brains were collected and sectioned, and a confocal microscope was used to image GFP-positive axon terminals within coronal brain tissue sections.
To examine the overlap between PAG-projecting POA neurons and Fos expression, Ai14 females first received a unilateral injection into the PAG of an AAV driving the retrograde expression of Cre-recombinase (AAVrg-pgk-Cre). Two weeks later, these females were given a 30-minute social interaction test. Ninety minutes after the test, subject females were perfused, brains were collected, and coronal brain sections containing the POA were collected for Fos immunohistochemistry as described above. Brain tissue sections were imaged with a 10x objective on a Zeiss 900 laser scanning confocal microscope, and POA neurons that were Fos-positive and tdTomato-positive were counted manually by trained observers.
Statistics
Two-sided statistical comparisons were used in all analyses (alpha = 0.05). The Shapiro-Wilk test was performed to analyze the normality of each data distribution, and non-parametric statistical tests were used for comparisons that included non-normally distributed data. No statistical methods were used to pre-determine sample size. Mice were only excluded from analysis in cases in which viral injections were not targeted accurately. Cages of mice were randomly assigned to either experimental or control groups. Video scoring of non-vocal social behaviors was conducted in a blinded fashion. Details of the statistical analyses used in this study are included in Table S1. All error bars show standard deviation unless otherwise noted.
Acknowledgements
Thanks to Frank Drank and other CARE staff for their excellent mouse husbandry. All experimental design schematics included in the figures were created with BioRender.com/b99z597.
Additional information
Author contributions
X.Z. and K.A.T. designed the experiments. X.Z. and Y.C. conducted the experiments. X.Z., Y.C., D.S., V.C., D.D., J.W.S., A.S., and K.A.T. analyzed data. X.Z. and K.A.T. wrote the manuscript, and all authors approved the final version.
Additional files
References
- Lesions to the medial preoptic nucleus differentially affect singing and nest box-directed behaviors within and outside of the breeding season in European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris)Behav Neurosci 120:1326–1336https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.120.6.1326
- Thirst-associated preoptic neurons encode an aversive motivational driveScience 357:1149–1155https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aan6747
- Effects of social isolation, re-socialization and age on cognitive and aggressive behaviors of Kunming mice and BALB/c miceAnim Sci J 88:798–806https://doi.org/10.1111/asj.12688
- The psychological effects of solitary confinement on prisoners in supermax units: reviewing what we know and recommending what should changeInt J Offender Ther Comp Criminol 52:622–640https://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X07309720
- Role of VTA dopamine neurons and neuroligin 3 in sociability traits related to nonfamiliar conspecific interactionNat Commun 9https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-05382-3
- The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivationPsychol Bull 117:497–529https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497
- A neural circuit for male sexual behavior and rewardCell 186:3862–3881https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2023.07.021
- Effects of medial preoptic lesions on male mouse ultrasonic vocalizations and copulatory behaviorBrain Res Bull 6:109–112https://doi.org/10.1016/s0361-9230(81)80033-0
- Chapter Three - Loneliness in the Modern Age: An Evolutionary Theory of Loneliness (ETL) In: Olson JM, editorAdvances in Experimental Social Psychology. Academic Press :127–197https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2018.03.003
- Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analysesPsychol Aging 21:140–151https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140
- Loneuness and Aggressive BehaviourJ Sol Pers Relat 2:243–252https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407585023001
- Flexible scaling and persistence of social vocal communicationNature 593:108–113https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03403-8
- The Social Motivation Theory of AutismTrends Cogn Sci 16:231–239https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007
- Evaluation of the Social Motivation Hypothesis of AutismJAMA Psychiatry 75:797–808https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.1100
- Responses and functions of dopamine in nucleus accumbens core during social behaviorsCell Rep 40https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2022.111246
- Temporal Evolution of Cortical Ensembles Promoting Remote Memory RetrievalNat Neurosci 22:460–469https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0318-7
- Social reward requires coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens oxytocin and serotoninNature 501:179–184https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12518
- Dissociation of hypothalamic effects on ultrasound production and copulationPhysiol Behav 46:299–307https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(89)90271-0
- Role of Calcr expressing neurons in the medial amygdala in social contact among femalesMol Brain 16https://doi.org/10.1186/s13041-023-00993-4
- Amylin-Calcitonin receptor signaling in the medial preoptic area mediates affiliative social behaviors in female miceNat Commun 13https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-022-28131-z
- Medial Preoptic Area Modulates Courtship Ultrasonic Vocalization in Adult Male MiceNeurosci Bull 35:697–708https://doi.org/10.1007/s12264-019-00365-w
- Communication calls produced by electrical stimulation of four structures in the guinea pig brainPLoS One 13https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0194091
- Natural neural projection dynamics underlying social behaviorCell 157:1535–1551https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2014.05.017
- Esr1+ cells in the ventromedial hypothalamus control female aggressionNat Neurosci 20:1580–1590https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4644
- Mental health outcomes of quarantine and isolation for infection prevention: a systematic umbrella review of the global evidenceEpidemiol Health 42https://doi.org/10.4178/epih.e2020038
- Social relationships and healthScience 241:540–545https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3399889
- An amygdala-to-hypothalamus circuit for social rewardNat Neurosci 24:831–842https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-021-00828-2
- Gating of social reward by oxytocin in the ventral tegmental areaScience 357:1406–1411https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aan4994
- Yearning for connection? Loneliness is associated with increased ventral striatum activity to close othersSoc Cogn Affect Neurosci 11:1096–1101https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv076
- The role of the periaqueductal grey in vocal behaviourBehav Brain Res 62:107–117https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(94)90017-5
- Distinct hypothalamic control of same- and opposite-sex mounting behaviour in miceNature 589:258–263https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2995-0
- Increasing aggression during the COVID-19 lockdownsJ Affect Disord Rep 5https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadr.2021.100163
- Functional circuit architecture underlying parental behaviourNature 556:326–331https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0027-0
- The neural circuitry of social homeostasis: Consequences of acute versus chronic social isolationCell 184:1500–1516https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2021.02.028
- Scalable control of mounting and attack by Esr1+ neurons in the ventromedial hypothalamusNature 509:627–632https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13169
- A Hypothalamic Circuit Underlying the Dynamic Control of Social HomeostasisbioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.05.19.540391
- Make war not love: The neural substrate underlying a state-dependent switch in female social behaviorNeuron 110:841–856https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2021.12.002
- Oxytocin, motivation and the role of dopaminePharmacol Biochem Behav 119:49–60https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2013.06.011
- Social isolation-induced aggression potentiates anxiety and depressive-like behavior in male mice subjected to unpredictable chronic mild stressPLoS One 6https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020955
- Lack of social touch alters anxiety-like and social behaviors in male miceStress 25:134–144https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2022.2047174
- Loneliness and Personality Profiles Involved in Bullying Victimization and Aggressive BehaviorSchool Ment Health 11:807–818https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-019-09328-y
- Social isolation stress-induced aggression in mice: a model to study the pharmacology of neurosteroidogenesisStress 8:85–93https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890500159022
- Dorsal Raphe Dopamine Neurons Represent the Experience of Social IsolationCell 164:617–631https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2015.12.040
- Hormonal gain control of a medial preoptic area social reward circuitNat Neurosci 20:449–458https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4487
- Age-dependent effects of social isolation on mesolimbic dopamine releaseExp Brain Res 240:2803–2815https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-022-06449-w
- Supermax Incarceration and RecidivismCriminology 47:1131–1166https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2009.00171.x
- Dopamine, Erectile Function and Male Sexual Behavior from the Past to the Present: A ReviewBrain Sci 12https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12070826
- Characteristics of exaggerated sexual behavior induced by electrical stimulation of the medial preoptic area in male ratsBrain Res 86:97–108https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(75)90641-1
- Circuit and synaptic organization of forebrain-to-midbrain pathways that promote and suppress vocalizationElife 9https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.63493
- Oxytocin neurons mediate the effect of social isolation via the VTA circuitseLife 11https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.73421
- Short-term isolation increases social interactions of male rats: a parametric analysisPhysiol Behav 29:819–825https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(82)90331-6
- Acute off-target effects of neural circuit manipulationsNature 528:358–363https://doi.org/10.1038/nature16442
- Social deprivation and play in ratsBehav Neural Biol 30:197–206https://doi.org/10.1016/s0163-1047(80)91077-8
- An Exploratory Study of COVID-19’s Impact on Psychological Distress and Antisocial Behavior Among Justice-Involved YouthCrime Delinquency 68:1271–1291https://doi.org/10.1177/00111287211054729
- Social Stimuli Induce Activation of Oxytocin Neurons Within the Paraventricular Nucleus of the Hypothalamus to Promote Social Behavior in Male MiceJ Neurosci 40:2282–2295https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1515-18.2020
- The role of motivation and reward neural systems in vocal communication in songbirdsFront Neuroendocrinol 33:194–209https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2012.04.002
- Lesions to the medial preoptic area affect singing in the male European starling (Sturnus vulgaris)Horm Behav 36:276–286https://doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.1999.1549
- Seasonal changes in courtship song and the medial preoptic area in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris)Horm Behav 38:250–261https://doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.2000.1623
- Fast dopamine release events in the nucleus accumbens of early adolescent ratsNeuroscience 176:296–307https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.12.016
- VTA dopamine neuron activity encodes social interaction and promotes reinforcement learning through social prediction errorNat Neurosci 25:86–97https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-021-00972-9
- Neural circuits and activity dynamics underlying sex-specific effects of chronic social isolation stressCell Rep 34https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2021.108874
- Oxytocin activation of neurons in ventral tegmental area and interfascicular nucleus of mouse midbrainNeuropharmacology 77:277–284https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.10.004
- Acute social isolation evokes midbrain craving responses similar to hungerNat Neurosci 23:1597–1605https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-00742-z
- Post-weaning social isolation induces abnormal forms of aggression in conjunction with increased glucocorticoid and autonomic stress responsesHorm Behav 60:28–36https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.02.003
- A Specialized Neural Circuit Gates Social Vocalizations in the MouseNeuron 103:459–472https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.025
- The “isolation syndrome” in micePsychopharmacologia 31:305–320https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00421275
- The neural mechanisms and circuitry of the pair bondNat Rev Neurosci 19:643–654https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-018-0072-6
- Ultrashort-range, high-frequency communication by female mice shapes social interactionsSci Rep 10https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-59418-0
- Medial preoptic area in mice is capable of mediating sexually dimorphic behaviors regardless of genderNat Commun 9https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-02648-0
- Effect of social isolation on stress-related behavioural and neuroendocrine state in the ratBehav Brain Res 152:279–295https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2003.10.015
- Long-term isolation stress in ratsScience 142https://doi.org/10.1126/science.142.3591.507
- Neural control of affiliative touch in prosocial interactionNature 599:262–267https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03962-w
- Biased Oxytocinergic Modulation of Midbrain Dopamine SystemsNeuron 95:368–384https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.003
- Transformations of neural representations in a social behavior networkNature 608:741–749https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-05057-6
- Social isolation rearing increases dopamine uptake and psychostimulant potency in the striatumNeuropharmacology 101:471–479https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2015.10.025
- The Neuropeptide Tac2 Controls a Distributed Brain State Induced by Chronic Social Isolation StressCell 173:1265–1279https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2018.03.037
- Sex- and context-dependent effects of acute isolation on vocal and non-vocal social behaviors in micePLoS One 16https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0255640
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
Copyright
© 2024, Zhao et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 650
- downloads
- 40
- citation
- 1
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.