Abstract
Cognitive flexibility requires both the encoding of task-relevant and the ignoring of task-irrelevant stimuli. While the neural coding of task-relevant stimuli is increasingly well understood, the mechanisms for ignoring task-irrelevant stimuli remain poorly understood. Here, we study how task performance and biological constraints jointly determine the coding of relevant and irrelevant stimuli in neural circuits. Using mathematical analyses and task-optimized recurrent neural networks, we show that neural circuits can exhibit a range of representational geometries depending on the strength of neural noise and metabolic cost. By comparing these results with recordings from primate prefrontal cortex (PFC) over the course of learning, we show that neural activity in PFC changes in line with a minimal representational strategy. Specifically, our analyses reveal that the suppression of dynamically irrelevant stimuli is achieved by activity-silent, sub-threshold dynamics. Our results provide a normative explanation as to why PFC implements an adaptive, minimal representational strategy.
Introduction
How systems solve complex cognitive tasks is a fundamental question in neuroscience and artificial intelligence 2–6. A key aspect of complex tasks is that they often involve multiple types of stimuli, some of which can even be irrelevant for performing the correct behavioural response 7–9 or predicting reward 10,11. Over the course of task exposure, subjects must typically identify which stimuli are relevant and which are irrelevant. Examples of irrelevant stimuli include those that are irrelevant at all times in a task 7,11–14 — which we refer to as static irrelevance — and stimuli that are relevant at some time points but are irrelevant at other times in a trial — which we refer to as dynamic irrelevance (e.g. as is often the case for context-dependent decision-making tasks 8,15–17). Although tasks involving irrelevant stimuli have been widely used, it remains an open question as to how different types of irrelevant stimuli should be represented, in combination with relevant stimuli, to enable optimal task performance.
One may naively think that statically irrelevant stimuli should always be suppressed. However, stimuli that are currently irrelevant may be relevant in a future task. Furthermore, it is unclear whether dynamically irrelevant stimuli should be suppressed at all since the information is ultimately needed by the circuit. It may therefore be beneficial for a neural circuit to represent irrelevant information as long as no unnecessary costs are incurred and task performance remains high. Several factors could have a strong impact on whether irrelevant stimuli affect task performance. For example, levels of neural noise in the circuit as well as energy constraints and the metabolic costs of overall neural activity 15,18–26 can affect how stimuli are represented in a neural circuit. Indeed, both noise and metabolic costs are factors that biological circuits must contend with 27–30. Despite these considerations, models of neural population codes, including hand-crafted models and optimized artificial neural networks, typically use only a very limited range of the values of such factors 4,8,19,26,31–35 (but also see 3,20). Therefore, despite the success of recent comparisons between neural network models and experimental recordings 4,8,19,22,26,31,34–36, we may only be recovering very few out of a potentially large range of different representational strategies that neural networks could exhibit 37.
One challenge for distinguishing between different representational strategies, particularly when analysing experimental recordings, is that some stimuli may simply be represented more strongly than others. In particular, we might expect stimuli to be strongly represented in cortex a priori if they have previously been important to the animal. Indeed, being able to represent a given stimulus when learning a new task is likely a prerequisite for learning whether it is relevant or irrelevant in that particular context 2,10. Previously, it has been difficult to distinguish between whether a given representation existed a priori or emerged as a consequence of learning because neural activity is typically only recorded after a task has already been learned. A more relevant question is how the representation changes over learning 11,38–41, which provides insights into how the specific task of interest affects the representational strategy used by an animal or artificial network 42.
To resolve these questions, we optimized recurrent neural networks on a task that involved two types of irrelevant stimuli. One feature of the stimulus was statically irrelevant, and another feature of the stimulus was dynamically irrelevant. We found that, depending on the neural noise level and metabolic cost that was imposed on the networks during training, a range of representational strategies emerged in the optimized networks, from maximal (representing all stimuli) to minimal (representing only relevant stimuli). We then compared the strategies of our optimized networks with learning-resolved recordings from the prefrontal cortex (PFC) of monkeys exposed to the same task. We found that the representational geometry of the neural recordings changed in line with the minimal strategy. Using a simplified model, we derived mathematically how the strength of relevant and irrelevant coding depends on the noise level and metabolic cost. We then confirmed our theoretical predictions in both our task-optimized networks and neural recordings. By reverse-engineering our task-optimized networks, we also found that activity-silent, sub-threshold dynamics led to the suppression of dynamically irrelevant stimuli, and we confirmed predictions of this mechanism in our neural recordings.
In summary, we provide a mechanistic understanding of how different representational strategies can emerge in both biological and artificial neural circuits over the course of learning in response to salient biological factors such as noise and metabolic costs. These results in turn explain why PFC appears to employ a minimal representational strategy by filtering out task-irrelevant information.
Results
A task involving relevant and irrelevant stimuli
We study a task used in previous experimental work that uses a combination of multiple, relevant and irrelevant stimuli 1 (Fig. 1a). The task consists of an initial ‘fixation’ period, followed by a ‘color’ period, in which one of two colors are presented. After this, in the ‘shape’ period, either a square or diamond shape is presented (while the color stimulus stays on), such that the width of the shape can be either thick or thin. After this, the stimuli disappear and reward is delivered according to an XOR structure between color and shape (Fig. 1b). Note that the width of the shape is not predictive of reward, and it is therefore an irrelevant stimulus dimension (Fig. 1b). As width information is always irrelevant when it is shown, its irrelevance is static. In contrast, color is relevant during the shape period but could be ignored during the color period without loss of performance. Hence its irrelevance is dynamic.
Due to the existence of multiple different forms of irrelevant stimuli, there exist multiple different representational strategies for a neural circuit solving the task in Fig. 1a. These representational strategies can be characterised by assessing the extent to which different stimuli are linearly decodable from neural population activity 10,14,43,44. We use linear decodability because it only requires the computation of simple weighted sums of neural responses, and as such, it is a widely accepted criterion for the usefulness of a neural representation 45. Moreover, while representational strategies can differ along several dimensions in this task (for example, the decodability of color or shape during the shape period — both of which are task-relevant 1), our main focus here is on the two dimensions that specifically control the representation of irrelevant stimuli. Therefore, depending on whether each of the irrelevant stimuli are linearly decodable during their respective period of irrelevance, we distinguish four different (extreme) strategies, ranging from a ‘minimal strategy’, in which the irrelevant stimuli are only weakly represented (Fig. 1c, bottom left; pink shading), to a ‘maximal strategy’, in which both irrelevant stimuli are strongly represented (Fig. 1c, top right; blue shading).
Stimulus representations in task-optimized recurrent neural networks
To understand the factors determining which representational strategy neural circuits employ to solve this task, we optimized recurrent neural networks to perform the task (Fig. 2a; Methods 1.2). The neural activities in these stochastic recurrent networks evolved according to
With
where x(t) = (x1(t), …, xN (t))⊤ corresponds to the vector of ‘subthreshold’ activities of the N = 50 neurons in the network, r(t) is their momentary firing rates which is a rectified linear function (ReLU) of the subthreshold activity, τ = 50 ms is the effective time constant, and h(t) denotes the inputs to the network encoding the three stimulus features as they become available over the course of the trial (Fig. 2a, bottom). The optimized parameters of the network were W, the recurrent weight matrix describing connection strengths between neurons in the network (Fig. 2a, top; middle), Win, the feedforward weight matrix describing connections from the stimulus inputs to the network (Fig. 2a, top; left; see also Methods 1.2), and b, a stimulusindependent bias. Importantly, σ is the standard deviation of the neural noise process (Fig. 2a, top; pale gray arrows; with η(t) being a sample from a Gaussian white noise process with mean 0 and variance 1), and as such represents a fundamental constraint on the operation of the network. The output of the network was given by
with optimized parameters Wout, the readout weights (Fig. 2a, top; right), and bout, a readout bias.
We optimized networks for a canonical cost function 20,32,33,35,46 (Fig. 2a, bottom; Methods 1.2.1):
The first term in Eq. 4 is a task performance term measuring the cross-entropy loss ℋ (c, z(t)) between the one-hot encoded target choice, c, and the network’s output probabilities, z(t), during the reward period, Tr (Fig. 2a, bottom; yellow shading). The second term in Eq. 4 is a firing rate regularization term. This regularization term can be interpreted as a form of energy or metabolic cost 18,24,31,46,47 measured across the whole trial, T, because it penalizes large overall firing rates. Therefore, optimizing this cost function encourages networks to not only solve the task, but to do so using low overall firing rates. How important it is for the network to keep firing rates low is controlled by the ‘regularization’ parameter λ. Critically, we used different noise levels σ and regularization strengths λ (Fig. 2a, highlighted in red) to examine how these two constraints affected the dynamical strategies employed by the optimized networks to solve the task and compared them to our four hypothesised representational strategies (Fig. 1c). We focused on these two parameters because they are both critical factors for real neural circuits and a priori can be expected to have important effects on the resulting circuit dynamics. For example, metabolic costs will constrain the overall level of firing rates that can be used to solve the task while noise levels directly affect how reliably stimuli can be encoded in the network dynamics. For the remainder of this section, we analyse representational strategies utilized by networks after training. In subsequent sections, we analyse learning-related changes in both our optimized networks and neural recordings.
We found that networks trained in a low noise–low firing rate regularization setting (which we denote by σl, λl) employed a maximal representational strategy (Fig. 2b, left column; pale blue shading). Trajectories in neural firing rate space diverged for the two different colors as soon as the color stimulus was presented (Fig. 2b, top left; blue and green trajectories from open gray circle to gray squares), which resulted in high color decoding during the color period (Fig. 2b, bottom left; turquoise line; Methods 1.3.1). During the shape period, the trajectories corresponding to each color diverged again (Fig. 2b, top left; trajectories from gray squares to filled gray circles), such that all stimuli were highly decodable, including width (Fig. 2b, bottom left; black line, Supplementary Fig. 1a). After removal of the stimuli during the reward period, trajectories converged to one of two parts of state space according to the XOR task rule — which is required for high performance (Fig. 2b, top left; trajectories from filled gray circles to colored squares and diamonds). Because early color and width were highly decodable in these networks trained with a low noise and low firing rate regularization, the dynamical strategy they employed corresponds to the maximal representational regime (cf. Fig. 1c, blue shading).
We next considered the setting of networks that solve this task while being exposed to a high level of neural noise (which we denote by σh, λl; Fig. 2b, middle column; purple shading). In this setting, we also observed neural trajectories that diverged during the color period (Fig. 2b, top middle; gray squares are separated), which yielded high color decoding during the color period (Fig. 2b, bottom middle; turquoise line). However, in contrast to networks with a low level of neural noise (Fig. 2b, left), width was poorly decodable during the shape period (Fig. 2b, bottom middle; black line). Therefore, for networks challenged with higher levels of neural noise, the irrelevant stimulus dimension of width is represented more weakly (cf. Fig. 1c, purple shading). A similar representational strategy was also observed in networks that were exposed to a low level of noise but a high level of firing rate regularization (Supplementary Fig. 1c, black lines).
Finally, we considered the setting of networks that solve this task while being exposed to medium levels of both neural noise and firing rate regularization (which we denote by σm, λm; Fig. 2b, right column; pink shading). In this setting, neural trajectories diverged only weakly during the color period (Fig. 2b, top middle; gray squares on-top of one another), which yielded relatively poor (non-ceiling) color decoding during the color period (Fig. 2b, bottom right; turquoise line). Nevertheless, color decoding was still above chance despite the neural trajectories strongly overlapping in the 2-dimensional state space plot in Fig. 2b, top right), because these trajectories became separable in the full-dimensional state space of these networks. Additionally, width decoding was also poor during the shape period (Fig. 2b, bottom right; black line). Therefore, networks that were challenged with increased levels of both neural noise and firing rate regularization employed dynamics in line with a minimal representational strategy by only weakly representing irrelevant stimuli (cf. Fig. 1c, pink shading).
To gain a more comprehensive picture of the full range of dynamical solutions that networks can exhibit, we performed a grid search over multiple different levels of noise and firing rate regularization. Nearly all parameter values allowed networks to achieve high performance (Fig. 2c, left), except when both the noise and regularization levels were high (Fig. 2c, left; parameter values above white line). Importantly, all of the dynamical regimes that we showed in Fig. 2b achieved similarly high performances (Fig. 2c, left; pale blue, purple, and pink dots).
When looking at early color decoding (defined as color decoding at the end of the color period; Fig. 2b, bottom left, turquoise arrow) and width decoding (defined as width decoding at the end of the shape period; Fig. 2b, bottom left, black arrow), we saw a consistent pattern of results. Early color decoding was high only when either the lowest noise level was used (Fig. 2c, middle; σ = 0.01 column) or when the lowest regularization level was used (Fig. 2c, middle; λ = 0.0005 row). In contrast, width decoding was high only when both the level of noise and regularization were small (Fig. 2c, right; bottom left corner). Otherwise, width decoding became progressively worse as either the noise or regularization level was increased and achieved values that were typically lower than the corresponding early color decoding (Fig. 2c, compare right with middle). This pattern becomes clearer when we plot width decoding against early color decoding (Fig. 2d). No set of parameters yielded higher width decodability compared to early color decodability (Fig. 2d, no data point above the identity line). This means that we never observed the fourth dynamical regime we hypothesized a priori, in which width decoding would be high and early color decoding would be low (Fig. 1c, top left). Therefore, information with static irrelevance (width) was more strongly suppressed compared to information whose irrelevance was dynamic (color). We also note that we never observed pure chance levels of decoding of color or width during stimulus presentation. This is likely because it is challenging for recurrent neural networks to strongly suppress their inputs and it may also be the case that other hyperparameter regimes more naturally lead to stronger suppression of inputs (we discuss this second possibility later; e.g. Fig. 5).
Comparing learning-related changes in stimulus representations in neural networks and primate lateral PFC
To understand the dynamical regime employed by PFC, we analyzed a data set 1 of multi-channel recordings from lateral prefrontal cortex (lPFC) in two monkeys exposed to the task shown in Fig. 1a. These recordings yielded 376 neurons in total across all recording sessions and both animals (Methods 1.1). Importantly, for understanding the direction in which neural geometries changed over learning, recordings commenced in the first session in which the animals were exposed to the task — that is, the recordings spanned the entirety of the learning process. For our analyses, we distinguished between the first half of recording sessions (Fig. 3a, gray; ‘early learning’) and the second half of recording sessions (Fig. 3a, black; ‘late learning’). A previous analysis of this dataset showed that, over the course of learning, the XOR representation of the task comes to dominate the dynamics during the late shape period of the task 1. Here however, we focus on relevant and irrelevant task variable coding during the stimulus periods and compare the recordings to the dynamics of task-optimized recurrent neural networks. Also, in line with the previous study 1, we do not examine the reward period of the task because the one-to-one relationship between XOR and reward in the data (which is not present in the models), will likely lead to trivial differences in XOR representations in the reward period between the data and models.
Similar to our analyses of our recurrent network models (Fig. 2b, bottom), we performed population decoding of the key task variables in the experimental data (Fig. 3a, Methods 1.3.1). We found that the decodability of the XOR relationship between task-relevant stimuli that determined task performance (Fig. 1b) significantly increased during the shape period over the course of learning, consistent with the animals becoming more familiar with the task structure (Fig. 3a, far left; compare gray and black lines). We also found that shape decodability during the shape period decreased slightly over learning (Fig. 3a, middle left; compare gray and black lines from gray square to gray circle), while color decodability during the shape period increased slightly (Fig. 3a, middle right; compare gray and black lines from gray square to gray circle). Importantly, however, color decodability significantly decreased during the color period (when it is ‘irrelevant’; Fig. 3a, middle right; compare gray and black lines), and width decodability significantly decreased during the shape period (Fig. 3a, far right; compare gray and black lines). Neural activities in lPFC thus appear to change in line with the minimal representational strategy over the course of learning, consistent with recurrent networks trained in the σm, λm regime (cf. Fig. 1c, pink and Fig. 2b, bottom, pink).
We then directly compared these learning-related changes in stimulus decodability from lPFC with those that we observed in our task-optimized recurrent neural networks (cf. Fig. 2). We found that the temporal decoding dynamics of networks trained with medium noise and firing rate regularization (σm, λm; Fig. 2b– d, pink shading and pink dots) exhibited decoding dynamics most similar to those that we observed in lPFC. Specifically, XOR decodability significantly increased after shape onset, consistent with the networks learning the task (Fig. 3b, far left; compare gray and black lines). We also found that shape and color decodability did not significantly change during the shape period (Fig. 3b, middle left and middle right; compare gray and black lines from gray square to gray circle). Importantly, however, color decodability significantly decreased during the color period (when it is ‘irrelevant’; Fig. 3b, middle right; compare gray and black lines from open gray circle to gray square), and width decodability significantly decreased during the shape period (Fig. 3a, far right; compare gray and black lines). Other noise and regularization settings yielded temporal decoding that displayed a poorer resemblance to the data (Supplementary Fig. 1). For example, σl, λl and σl, λh networks exhibited almost no changes in decodability during the color and shape periods (Supplementary Fig. 1a,c) and σh, λl networks exhibited increased XOR, shape, and color decodability at all times after stimulus onset while width decodability decreased during the shape period (Supplementary Fig. 1b). We also found that if regularization is driven to very high levels, color and shape decoding becomes weak during the shape period while XOR decoding remains high (Supplementary Fig. 1d). Therefore such networks effectively perform a pure XOR computation during the shape period.
We also note that the σm, λm model does not perfectly match the decoding dynamics seen in the data. For example, although not significant, we observed a decrease in shape decoding and an increase in color decoding in the data during the shape period whereas the model only displayed a slight (non significant) increase in decodability of both shape and color during the same time period. These differences may be due to fundamentally different ways that brains encode sensory information upstream of PFC, compared to the more simplistic abstract sensory inputs used in models (see Discussion).
To systematically compare learning-related changes in the data and models, we analyzed time periods when there were significant changes in decoding in the data over the course of learning (Fig. 3a, horizontal black bars). This yielded a substantial increase in XOR decoding during the shape period, and substantial decreases in color and width decoding during the color and shape periods, respectively, in the data (Fig. 3c, far left column). During the same time periods in the models, networks in the σl, λl regime exhibited no changes in decoding (Fig. 3c, middle left column; blue shading). In contrast, networks in the σh, λl regime exhibited substantial increases in XOR and color decoding, and a substantial decrease in width decoding (Fig. 3c, middle right column; purple shading). Finally, in line with the data, networks in the σm, λm regime exhibited a substantial increase in XOR decoding, and substantial decreases in both color and width decoding (Fig. 3c, middle right column; pink shading).
In addition to studying changes in traditional decoding, we also studied learning-related changes in ‘cross-generalized decoding’ which provides a measure of how factorized the representational geometry is across stimulus conditions 10 (Supplementary Fig. 2). (For example, for evaluating cross-generalized decoding for color, a color decoder trained on square trials would be tested on diamond trials, and vice versa 10; Methods 1.3.1.) Using this measure, we found that changes in decoding were generally more extreme over learning and that models and data bore a stronger resemblance to one another compared with traditional decoding. Specifically, all models and the data showed a strong increase in XOR decoding (Fig. 3d, top row, ‘XOR’) and a strong decrease in width decoding (Fig. 3d, bottom row, ‘width’). However, only the data and σm, λm networks showed a decrease in color decoding (Fig. 3d, compare middle far left and middle far right), whereas σl, λl networks showed no change in color decoding (Fig. 3d, middle left; blue shading) and σh, λl networks showed an increase in color decoding (Fig. 3d, middle right; purple shading).
Beside studying the effects of input noise and firing rate regularization, we also examined the effects of different strengths of the initial stimulus input connections prior to training 15. In line with previous studies 3,20,35,46,48, for all of our previous results, the initial input weights were set to small random values prior to training (Methods 1.2.1). We found that changing these weights had similar effects to changing neural noise (with the opposite sign). Specifically, when input weights were set to 0 before training, initial decoding was at chance levels and only increased with learning for XOR, shape, and color (whereas width decoding hardly changed with learning and remained close to chance levels; Supplementary Fig. 3) — analogously to the σh, λl regime (Supplementary Fig. 1b). In contrast, when input weights were set to large random values prior to training, initial decoding was at ceiling levels and did not significantly change over learning during the color and shape periods for any of the task variables (Supplementary Fig. 4) — similar to what we found in the σl, λl regime (Supplementary Fig. 1a). Thus, neither extremely small nor extremely large initial input weights were consistent with the data that exhibited both increases and decreases in decodability of task variables over learning (Fig. 3a).
Theoretical predictions for strength of relevant and irrelevant stimulus coding
To gain a theoretical understanding of how irrelevant stimuli should be processed in a neural circuit, we performed a mathematical analysis of a minimal linear model that only included a single relevant stimulus and a single statically irrelevant stimulus (Supplement A1). Although this analysis applies to a simpler task compared with that faced by our neural networks and animals, crucially it still allows us to understand how relevant and irrelevant coding depend on noise and metabolic constraints. Our mathematical analysis suggested that the effects of noise and firing rate regularization on the performance and metabolic cost of a network can be understood via three key aspects of its representation: the strength of neural responses to the relevant stimulus (‘relevant coding’), the strength of responses to the irrelevant stimulus (‘irrelevant coding’), and the overlap between the population responses to relevant and irrelevant stimuli (‘overlap’; Fig. 4a). In particular, maximizing task performance (i.e. the decodability of the relevant stimulus) required relevant coding to be strong, irrelevant coding to be weak, and the overlap between the two to be small (Fig. 4b, top; Supplement A1.3). This ensures that the irrelevant stimulus interferes minimally with the relevant stimulus. In contrast, to reduce a metabolic cost (such as we considered previously in our optimized recurrent networks, see Eq. 4 and Fig. 2), both relevant and irrelevant coding should be weak (Fig. 4b, bottom; Supplement A1.4).
In combination, when decoding performance and metabolic cost are jointly optimized, as in our taskoptimized recurrent networks (Fig. 2), our theoretical analyses suggested that performance should decrease with both the noise level σ and the strength of firing rate regularization λ in an approximately interchangeable way, and metabolic cost should increase with σ but decrease with λ (Supplement A1.5). We also found that the strength of relevant coding should decrease with λ, but its dependence on σ was more nuanced. For small σ, the performance term effectively dominates the metabolic cost (Fig. 4b, top) and the strength of relevant coding should increase with σ. However, if σ is too high, the strength of relevant coding starts decreasing otherwise a disproportionately high metabolic cost must be incurred to achieve high performance (Fig. 4c,d). Our mathematical analysis also suggested that irrelevant coding and relevant– irrelevant overlap could in principle depend on the noise and metabolic cost strength — particularly if performing noisy optimization where the curvature of the cost function can be relatively shallow around the minimum (Supplement A1.5). Therefore, practically, we also expect that irrelevant coding should be mostly dependent (inversely) on λ, but relevant–irrelevant overlap should mostly depend on σ (Supplement A1.6). These theoretical predictions were confirmed by our recurrent neural network simulations (Fig. 4e–g).
We next measured, in both recorded and simulated population responses, the three aspects of population responses that our theory identified as being key in determining the performance and metabolic cost of a network (Fig. 4a; Methods 1.3.2). We found a close correspondence in the learning-related changes of these measures between our lPFC recordings (Fig. 4h) and optimized recurrent networks (Fig. 4i). In particular, we found that the strength of relevant (XOR) coding increased significantly over the course of learning (Fig. 4h,i, left; red). The strength of irrelevant (width) coding decreased significantly over the course of learning (Fig. 4h,i, left; black), such that it became significantly smaller than the strength of relevant coding (Fig. 4h,i, left; compare red and black at ‘late learning’). Finally, relevant and irrelevant directions were always strongly orthogonal in neural state space, and the level of orthogonality did not significantly change with learning (Fig. 4h,i, right). Although we observed no learning-related changes in overlap, it may be that for stimuli that are more similar than the relevant and irrelevant features we studied here (XOR and width), the overlap between these features may decrease over learning rather than simply remaining small.
Activity-silent, sub-threshold dynamics lead to the suppression of dynamically irrelevant stimuli
While the representation of statically irrelevant stimuli can be suppressed by simply weakening the input connections conveying information about it to the network, the suppression of dynamically irrelevant stimuli requires a mechanism that alters the dynamics of the network (since this information ultimately needs to be used by the network to achieve high performance). To gain some intuition about this mechanism, we first analyzed 2-neuron networks trained on the task. To demonstrate the basic mechanism of suppression of dynamically irrelevant stimuli (i.e. weak early color coding), we compared the σl, λl (Fig. 5a) and σm, λm (Fig. 5b) regimes for these networks, as these corresponded to the minimal and maximal amount of suppression of dynamically irrelevant stimuli (Fig. 2c).
We examined trajectories of sub-threshold neural activity x(t) (cf. Eq. 1) in the full 2-neuron state space (Fig. 5a,b, blue and green curves). We distinguished between the negative quadrant of state space, which corresponds to the rectified part of the firing rate nonlinearity (Fig. 5a,b, bottom left gray quadrant; cf. Eq. 2), and the rest of state space. Importantly, if sub-threshold activity lies within the negative quadrant at some time point, both neurons in the network have zero firing rate and consequently a decoder cannot decode any information from the firing rate activity and the network exhibits no metabolic cost (cf. Eq. 4). Therefore, we reasoned that color inputs may drive sub-threshold activity so that it lies purely in the negative quadrant of state space so that no metabolic cost is incurred during the color period (akin to nonlinear gating 15,49). Later in the trial, when these color inputs are combined with the shape inputs, activity may then leave the negative quadrant of state space so that the network can perform the task.
We found that for the σl, λl regime, there was typically at least one set of stimulus conditions for which subthreshold neural activity evolved outside the negative quadrant of state space during any task period (Fig. 5a). Consequently, color was decodable to a relatively high level during the color period (Supplementary Fig. 5a, middle right) and this network produced a relatively high metabolic cost throughout the task (Fig. 5c, blue line). In contrast, for networks in the σm, λm regime, the two color inputs typically drove neural activity into the negative quadrant of state space during the color period (Fig. 5b, left). Therefore, during the color period, the network produced zero firing rate activity (Fig. 5c, pink line from open gray circle to gray square). Consequently, color was poorly decodable (Supplementary Fig. 5b, middle right; black line) and the network incurred no metabolic cost during the color period. Thus, color information was represented in a sub-threshold, ‘activity-silent’ 50 state during the color period. However, during the shape and reward periods later in the trial, the color inputs, now in combination with the shape inputs, affected the firing rate dynamics and the neural trajectories explored the full state space in a similar manner to the σl, λl network (Fig. 5b, middle and right panels). Indeed, we also found that color decodability increased substantially during the shape period in the σm, λm network (Supplementary Fig. 5b, middle right; black line). This demonstrates how color inputs can cause no change in firing rates during the color period when they are irrelevant, and yet these same inputs can be utilized later in the trial to enable high task performance (Supplementary Fig. 5a,b, far left). While we considered individual example networks in Fig. 5a–c, color inputs consistently drove neural activity into the negative quadrant of state space across repeated training of σm, λm networks but not for σl, λl networks (Fig. 5d).
Next, we performed the same analysis as Fig. 5d on the large (50-neuron) networks that we studied previously (Figs. 2–4). Similar to the 2-neuron networks, we found that large networks in the σl, λl regime did not exhibit more negative color inputs than would be expected by chance (Fig. 5e, pale blue highlighted point) — consistent with the high early color decoding we found previously in these networks (Fig. 2c, middle, pale blue highlighted point). However, when increasing either the noise or regularization level, the proportion of negative color inputs increased above chance such that for the highest noise and regularization level, nearly all color inputs were negative (Fig. 5e). We also found a strong negative correlation between the proportion of negative color input and the level of early color decoding that we found previously in Fig. 2c (Supplementary Fig. 5c, r = − 0.9, p < 10−9). This suggests that color inputs that drive neural activity into the rectified part of the firing rate nonlinearity, and thus generate purely sub-threshold activity-silent dynamics, is the mechanism that generates weak early color coding during the color period in these networks. We also examined whether these results generalized to networks that use a sigmoid (as opposed to a ReLU) nonlinearity. To do this, we trained networks with a tanh nonlinearity (shifted for a meaningful comparison with ReLU, so that the lower bound on firing rates was 0, rather than− 1) and found qualitatively similar results to the ReLU networks. In particular, color inputs drove neural activity towards 0 firing rate during the color period in σm, λm networks but not in σl, λl networks (Supplementary Fig. 6, compare a and b), which resulted in a lower metabolic cost during the color period for σm, λm networks compared to σl, λl networks (Supplementary Fig. 6c). This was reflected in color inputs being more strongly negative in σm, λm networks compared to σl, λl networks which only showed chance levels of negative color inputs (Supplementary Fig. 6d).
We next sought to test whether this mechanism could also explain the decrease in color decodability over learning that we observed in the lPFC data. To do this, we measured the magnitude of firing rates in the fixation, color, and shape periods for both early and late learning (note that the magnitude of firing rates coincides with our definition of the metabolic cost; cf. Eq. 4 and Fig. 5c). To decrease metabolic cost over learning, we would expect two changes: firing rates should decrease with learning and firing rates should not significantly increase from the fixation to the color period after learning (Fig. 5c, pink line). Indeed, we found that firing rates decreased significantly over the course of learning in all task periods (Fig. 5f, far left; compare gray and black lines), and this decrease was most substantial during the fixation and color periods (Fig. 5f, far left). We also found that after learning, firing rates during the color period were not significantly higher than during the fixation period (Fig. 5f, far left; compare black error bars during fixation and color periods). During the shape period however, firing rates increased significantly compared to those during the fixation period (Fig. 5f, far left; compare black error bars during fixation and shape periods). Therefore, the late learning dynamics of the data are in line with what we saw for the optimized 2-neuron σm, λm network (Fig. 5c, pink line).
We then compared these results from our neural recordings with the results from our large networks trained in different noise and regularization regimes. We found that for σl, λl networks, over the course of learning, firing rates decreased slightly during the fixation and color periods but actually increased during the shape period (Fig. 5f, middle left pale blue shading; compare gray and black lines). Additionally, after learning, firing rates increased between fixation and color periods and between color and shape periods (Fig. 5f, middle left pale blue shading; compare black error bars). For σh, λl networks, over the course of learning, we found that firing rates did not significantly change during the fixation and color periods but increased significantly during the shape period (Fig. 5f, middle right purple shading; compare gray and black lines). Furthermore, after learning, firing rates did not change significantly between fixation and color periods but did increase significantly between color and shape periods (Fig. 5f, middle right purple shading; compare black error bars). Finally, for the σm, λm networks, we found a pattern of results that was most consistent with the data. Firing rates decreased over the course of learning in all task periods (Fig. 5f, far right pink shading; compare gray and black lines) and the decrease in firing rates was most substantial during the fixation and color periods. After learning, firing rates did not change significantly from fixation to color periods but did increase significantly during the shape period (Fig. 5f, far right; compare black error bars).
To investigate whether these findings depended on our random network initialization prior to training, we also compared late learning firing rates to firing rates that resulted from randomly shuffling the color, shape, and width inputs (which emulates alternative tasks where different combinations of color, shape, and width are relevant). For example, the relative strengths of the 3 inputs to the network prior to training on this task may affect how the firing rates change over learning. Under this control, we also found that σm, λm networks bore a close resemblance to the data (Supplementary Fig. 5d, compare black lines to blue lines).
Discussion
Comparing the neural representations of taskoptimized networks with those observed in experimental data has been particularly fruitful in recent years 4,8,19,26,31,34–36. However, networks are typically optimized using only a very limited range of hyperparameter values 4,8,19,26,31–35. Instead, here we showed that different settings of key, biologically relevant hyperparameters such as noise and metabolic costs, can yield a variety of qualitatively different dynamical regimes that bear varying degrees of similarity with experimental data. In general, we found that increasing levels of noise and firing rate regularization led to increasing amounts of irrelevant information being filtered out in the networks. Indeed, filtering out of task-irrelevant information is a well-known property of the PFC and has been observed in a variety of tasks 7,12,31,38,49,51–53. We provide a mechanistic understanding of the specific conditions that lead to stronger filtering of task-irrelevant information. We predict that these results should also generalize to richer, more complex cognitive tasks that may, for example, require context switching 15,38,52 or invoke working memory 7,13,52. Indeed, filtering out of taskirrelevant information in the PFC has been observed in such tasks 7,13,15,38,52,54.
Our results are also likely a more general finding of neural circuits that extend beyond the PFC. In line with this, it has previously been shown that strongly regularized neural network models trained to reproduce monkey muscle activities during reaching bore a stronger resemblance to neural recordings from primary motor cortex compared to unregularized models 19. In related work on motor control, recurrent networks controlled by an optimal feedback controller recapitulated key aspects of experimental recordings from primary motor cortex (such as orthogonality between preparatory and movement neural activities) when the control input was regularized 24. Additionally, regularization of neural firing rates, and its natural biological interpretation as a metabolic cost, has recently been shown to be a key ingredient for the formation of grid cell-like response profiles in artificial networks 18,22.
By showing that PFC representations changed in line with a minimal representational strategy, our results are in line with various studies showing lowdimensional representations under a variety of tasks in the PFC and other brain regions 13,15,31,55,56. This is in contrast to several previous observations of highdimensional neural activity in PFC 2,10. Both highand low-dimensional regimes confer distinct yet useful benefits: high-dimensional representations allow many behavioural readouts to be generated, thereby enabling highly flexible behaviour 2,15,26,57–59, whereas low-dimensional representations are more robust to noise and allow for generalization across different stimuli 15,26,59. These two different representational strategies have previously been studied in models by setting the initial network weights to either small values (to generate low-dimensional ‘rich’ representations) or large values 15 (to generate high-dimensional ‘lazy’ representations). However, in contrast to previous approaches, we studied the more biologically plausible effects of firing rate regularization (i.e. a metabolic cost; see also the supplement of Ref. 15) on the network activities over the course of learning and compared them to learning-related changes in PFC neural activities. Firing rate regularization will cause neural activities to wax and wane as networks are exposed to new tasks depending upon the stimuli that are currently relevant. In line with this, it is conceivable that prolonged exposure to a task that has a limited number of stimulus conditions, some of which can even be generalized over (as was the case for our task), encourages more low dimensional dynamics to form 1,15,48,59–61. In contrast, tasks that use a rich variety of stimuli (that may even dynamically change over the task 4,6,62), and which do not involve generalization across stimulus conditions, may more naturally lead to high-dimensional representations 2,26,48,59,61,63. It would therefore be an important future question to understand how our results also depend on the task being studied as some tasks may more naturally lead to the ‘maximal’ representational regime 26,48,61 (Figs. 1– 3 and 5, blue shading).
A previous analysis of the same dataset that we studied here focused on the late parts of the trial 1. In particular, they found that the final result of the computation needed for the task, the XOR operation between color and shape, emerges and eventually comes to dominate lPFC representations over the course of learning in the late shape period 1. Our analysis goes beyond this by studying mechanisms of suppression of both static and dynamically irrelevant stimuli across all task periods and how different levels of neural noise and metabolic cost can lead to qualitatively different representations of irrelevant stimuli in task-optimized recurrent networks. Other previous studies focused on characterizing the representation of several taskrelevant 2,10,14 (and, in some cases, -irrelevant 15) variables over the course of individual trials. Characterizing how key aspects of neural representations change over the course of learning, as we did here, offers unique opportunities for studying the functional objectives shaping neural representations 42.
There were several aspects of the data that were not well captured by our models. For example, during the shape period, decodability of shape decreased while decodability of color increased (although not significantly) in our neural recordings (Fig. 3a). These differences in changes in decoding may be due to fundamentally different ways that brains encode sensory information upstream of PFC, compared to our models. For example, shape and width are both geometric features of the stimulus, whose encoding is differentiated from that of color already at early stages of visual processing 64. Such a hierarchical representation of inputs may automatically lead to the (un)learning about the relevance of width (which the σm, λm model reproduced) generalizing to shape, but not to color. In contrast, inputs in our model used a non-hierarchical one-hot encoding (Fig. 2), which did not allow for such generalization. Moreover, in the data, we may expect width to be a priori more strongly represented than color or shape because it is a much more potent sensory feature. In line with this, in our neural recordings, we found that width was very strongly represented in early learning compared to the other stimulus features (Fig. 3a, far right) and width always yielded high cross-generalized decoding — even after learning (Supplementary Fig. 2a, far right). Nevertheless, studying changes in decoding over learning, rather than absolute decoding levels, allowed us to focus on features of learning that do not depend on the details of upstream sensory representations of stimuli. Future studies could incorporate aspects of sensory representations that we ignored here by using stimulus inputs with which the model more faithfully reproduces the experimentally observed initial decodability of stimulus features.
In line with previous studies 3,18–20,24,31–33,35,46,47, we operationalized metabolic cost in our models through L2 firing rate regularization. This cost penalizes high overall firing rates. There are however alternative conceivable ways to operationalize a metabolic cost; for example L1 firing rate regularization has been used previously when optimizing neural networks and promotes more sparse neural firing 3. Interestingly, although our L2 is generally conceived to be weaker than L1 regularization, we still found that it encouraged the network to use purely sub-threshold activity in our task. The regularization of synaptic weights may also be biologically relevant 3 because synaptic transmission uses the most energy in the brain compared to other processes 65,66. Additionally, even subthreshold activity could be regularized as it also consumes energy (although orders of magnitude less than spiking 67). Therefore, future work will be needed to examine how different metabolic costs affect the dynamics of task-optimized networks.
We build on several previous studies that have also analysed learning-related changes in PFC activity 1,38,39,63 — although these studies typically used reversal-learning paradigms in which animals are already highly task proficient and the effects of learning and task switching are inseparable. For example, in a rule-based categorization task in which the categorization rule changed after learning an initial rule, neurons in mouse PFC adjusted their selectivity depending on the rule such that currently irrelevant information was not represented 38. Similarly, neurons in rat PFC transition rapidly from representing a familiar rule to representing a completely novel rule through trial and error learning 39. Additionally, the dimensionality of PFC representations was found to increase as monkeys learned the value of novel stimuli 63. Importantly, however, PFC representations did not distinguish between novel stimuli when they were first chosen. It was only once the value of the stimuli were learned that their representations in PFC were distinguishable 63. These results are consistent with our results where we found poor XOR decoding during the early stages of learning which then increased over learning as the monkeys learned the rewarded conditions (Fig. 3a, far left). However, we also observed high decoding of width during early learning which was not predictive of reward (Fig. 3a, far right). One key distinction between our study and that of Ref. 63, is that our recordings commenced from the first trial the monkeys were exposed to the task. In contrast, in Ref. 63, the monkeys were already highly proficient at the task and so the neural representation of their task was already likely strongly task specific by the time recordings were taken.
In line with our approach here, several recent studies have also examined the effects of different hyperparameter settings on the solution that optimized networks exhibit. One study found that decreasing regularization on network weights led to more sequential dynamics in networks optimized to perform working memory tasks 20. Another study found that the number of functional clusters that a network exhibits does not depend strongly on the strength of (L1 rate or weight) regularization, but did depend upon whether the single neuron nonlinearity saturates at high firing rates 3. It has also been shown that networks optimized to perform path integration can exhibit a range of different properties, from grid cell-like receptive fields to distinctly non grid cell-like receptive fields, depending upon biologically relevant hyperparameters — including noise and regularization 18,22,37. Indeed, in addition to noise and regularization, various other hyperparameters have also been shown to affect the representational strategy used by a circuit, such as the firing rate nonlinearity 3,18,37 and network weight initialization 15,37. It is therefore becoming increasingly clear that analysing the interplay between learning and biological constraints will be key for understanding the computations that various brain regions perform.
Acknowledgements
This work was funded by the Wellcome Trust (Investigator Award in Science 212262/Z/18/Z to M.L., Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship 215909/Z/19/Z to J.S., and award 101092/Z/13/Z to M.K., M.K., and J.D.), the Human Frontier Science Programme (Research Grant RGP0044/2018 to M.L.), the Medical Research Council UK Program (MC_UU_00030/7 to M.K., M.K., and J.D.), the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (award BB/M010732/1 to M.G.S.), a GatesCambridge scholarship (to K.T.J.), a Clarenden scholarship and Saven European scholarship (to M.W.), and the James S. McDonnell Foundation (award 220020405 to M.G.S.). For the purpose of open access, the authors have applied a CC-BY public copyright licence to any author accepted manuscript version arising from this submission. We thank Christopher Summerfield for useful feedback and detailed comments on the manuscript.
Competing Interests statement
The authors declare no competing interests.
Code availability
All code was custom written in Python using NumPy, SciPy, Matplotlib, Scikit-learn, and Tensorflow libraries. Code is available in the following repository: https://github.com/jakepstroud.
1 Methods
1.1 Experimental materials and methods
Experimental methods have been described previously 1. Briefly, two adult male rhesus macaques (monkey 1 and monkey 2) performed a passive object–association task (Fig. 1a,b; see the main text ‘A task involving relevant and irrelevant stimuli’ for a description of the task). Neural recordings commenced from the first session the animals were exposed to the task. All trials with fixation errors were excluded. The dataset contained on average 237.9 (s.d. = 23.9) and 104.8 (s.d. = 2.3) trials for each of the 8 conditions for monkeys 1 and 2, respectively. Data were recorded from the ventral and dorsal lateral PFC over a total of 27 daily sessions across both monkeys which yielded 146 and 230 neurons for monkey 1 and monkey 2, respectively. To compute neural firing rates, we convolved binary spike trains with a Gaussian kernel with a standard deviation of 50 ms. In order to characterize changes in neural dynamics over learning, analyses were performed separately on the first half of sessions (‘early learning’; 9 and 5 sessions from monkey 1 and monkey 2, respectively) and the second half of sessions (‘late learning’; 8 and 5 sessions from monkey 1 and monkey 2, respectively; Figs. 3–5 and Supplementary Fig. 2a).
1.2 Neural network models
The dynamics of our simulated networks evolved according to Eqs. 1 and 2 and are repeated here:
where x(t) = (x1(t), …, xN (t))⊤ corresponds to the vector of ‘subthreshold’ activities of the N neurons in the network, r(t) is their momentary firing rates, obtained as a rectified linear function (ReLU) of their subthreshold activities (Eq. 6; except for the networks of Supplementary Fig. 6 in which we used a tanh nonlinearity to examine the generalizability of our results), τ = 50 ms is the effective time constant, W is the recurrent weight matrix, h(t) denotes the total stimulus input, b is a stimulus-independent bias, σ is the standard deviation of the neural noise process, η(t) is a sample from a Gaussian white noise process with mean 0 and variance 1, and are color, shape, and width input weights, respectively, and hc(t), hs(t), and hw(t) are one-hot encodings of color, shape, and width inputs, respectively.
All simulations started at t0 = − 0.5 s and lasted until tmax = 1.5 s, and consisted of a fixation (− 0.5–0 s), color (0–0.5 s), shape (0.5–1 s), and reward (1–1.5 s) period (Fig. 1a and Fig. 2a). The initial condition of neural activity was set to x(t0) = 0. In line with the task, elements of hc(t) were set to 0 outside the color and shape periods, and elements of both hs(t) and hw(t) were set to 0 outside the shape period. All networks used N = 50 neurons (except for Fig. 5a–d and Supplementary Fig. 5a,b which used N = 2 neurons). We solved the dynamics of Eqs. 5-7 using a first-order Euler–Maruyama approximation with a discretization time step of 1 ms.
1.2.1 Network optimization
Choice probabilities were computed through a linear readout of network activities:
where Wout are the readout weights and bout is a readout bias. To measure network performance, we used a canonical cost function 3,20,32,33,35,46 (Eqs. 3 and 4). We repeat the cost function from the main text here:
where the first term is a task performance term which consists of the cross-entropy loss between the one-hot encoded target choice, c (based on the stimuli of the given trial, as defined by the task rules, Fig. 1b), and the network’s readout probabilities, z(t) (Eq. 8). Note that we measure total classification performance (cross entropy loss) during the reward period (integral in the first term runs from t = 1.0 to t = 1.5; Fig. 2a, bottom; yellow shading), as appropriate for the task. The second term in Eq. 9 is a widely-used 3,18–20,24,32,33,35,46 L2 regularization term (with strength λ) applied to the neural firing rates throughout the trial (integral in the second term runs from t = −0.5 to t = 1.5).
We initialized the free parameters of the network (the elements of , and ) by sampling (independently) from a normal distribution of mean 0 and standard deviation . There were two exceptions to this: we also investigated the effects of initializing the elements of the input weights to either 0
(Supplementary Fig. 3) or sampling their elements from a normal distribution of mean 0 and standard deviation (Supplementary Fig. 4). After initialization, we optimized these parameters using gradient descent with Adam 68, where gradients were obtained from backpropagation through time. We used a learning rate of 0.001 and trained networks for 1000 iterations using a batch size of 10. For each noise σ and regularization level λ (see Table 1), we optimized 10 networks with different random initializations of the network parameters.
1.3 Analysis methods
Here we describe methods that we used to analyse neural activities. Whenever applicable, the same processing and analysis steps were applied to both experimentally recorded and model simulated data. All neural firing rates were sub-sampled at a 10-ms resolution and, unless stated otherwise, we did not trial-average firing rates before performing analyses. Analyses were either performed at every time point in the trial (Fig. 2b, Fig. 3a,b, Fig. 5a–c, Supplementary Figs. 1–4, and Supplementary Fig. 5a,b), at the end of either the color (Fig. 2c,d, and Supplementary Fig. 5c; ‘early color decoding’) or shape periods (Fig. 2c,d, ‘width decoding’), during time periods of significant changes in decoding over learning in the data (Fig. 3c,d), during the final 100 ms of the shape period (Fig. 4e–i), or during the final 100 ms of the fixation, color, and shape periods (Fig. 5f, and Supplementary Fig. 5d).
1.3.1 Linear decoding
For decoding analyses (Fig. 2b–d, Fig. 3, Supplementary Figs. 1–4, and Supplementary Fig. 5a–c), we fitted decoders using linear support vector machines to decode the main task variables: color, shape, width, and the XOR between color and shape. We measured decoding performance in a cross-validated way, using separate sets of trials to train and test the decoders, and we show results averaged over 10 random 1:1 train:test splits. For firing rates resulting from simulated neural networks, we used 10 trials for both the train and test splits. Chance level decoding was always 0.5 as all stimuli were binary.
For showing changes in decoding over learning (Fig. 3c,d), we identified time periods of significant changes in decoding during the color and shape periods in the data (Fig. 3a, horizontal black bars; see Methods 1.4), and show the mean change in decoding during these time periods for both the data and models (Fig. 3c, horizontal black lines). We used the same time periods when showing changes in cross-generalized decoding over learning (Fig. 3d, see below) For cross-generalized decoding 10 (Fig. 3d and Supplementary Fig. 2), we used the same decoding approach as described above, except that cross validation was performed across trials corresponding to different stimulus conditions. Specifically, following the approach outlined in Ref. 10, because there were 3 binary stimuli in our task (color, shape, and width), there were 8 different trial conditions (Fig. 1b). Therefore, for each task variable (we focused on color, shape, width, and the XOR between color and shape), there were 6 different ways of choosing 2 of the 4 conditions corresponding to each of the two possible stimuli for that task variable (e.g. color 1 vs. color 2). For example, when training a decoder to decode color, there were 6 different ways of choosing 2 conditions corresponding to color 1, and 6 different ways of choosing 2 conditions corresponding to color 2 (the remaining 4 conditions were then used for testing the decoder). Therefore, for each task variable, there were 6 × 6 = 36 different ways of creating training and testing sets that corresponded to different stimulus conditions. We then took the mean decoding accuracy we obtained across all 36 different training and testing sets.
1.3.2 Measuring stimulus coding strength
In line with our mathematical analysis (Supplement A1, and in particular Eq. S1) and in line with previous studies 8,48,69, to measure the strength of stimulus coding for relevant and irrelevant stimuli, we fitted the following linear regression model
where r is size K × N (where K is the number of trials) and corresponds to the neural firing rates, X is size K× 3 where the first column is all 1 (thus encoding the mean firing rate of each neuron across all conditions) and elements of the final two columns are either − 0.5 or 0.5 depending upon whether trials correspond to XOR 1 or XOR 2 (relevant) conditions (column 2) or whether trials correspond to width 1 or width 2 (irrelevant) conditions (column 3). The coefficients to be fitted (β) is size 3 × N and has the following structure β = [μ, Δrrel, Δrirrel]⊤ where μ is the mean across all conditions, Δrrel are the coefficients corresponding to relevant (XOR) conditions, and Δrirrel are the coefficients corresponding to irrelevant (width) conditions. Finally, ϵ is size K× N and contains the residuals. Note that calculating the mean difference in firing rate between the two relevant conditions and between the two irrelevant conditions would yield identical estimations of Δrrel and Δrirrel because our stimuli are binary. (We also fitted decoders to decode either relevant or irrelevant conditions and extracted their coefficients to instead obtain Δrrel and Δrirrel and obtained near-identical results.)
We then calculated the Euclidean norm of both Δrrel and Δrirrel normalized by the number of neurons and and the absolute value of the normalized dot product (overlap) between them Fig. 4e–i ). For our neural recordings (Fig. 4h), we calculated these quantities separately for 10 non-overlapping splits of the data. For our neural networks (Fig. 4e–g,i, ), we calculated these quantities separately for 10 different networks.
1.3.3 Measuring the magnitude of neural firing rates
We first calculated the firing rate for each condition and time point and then calculated the mean (across conditions and time points) Euclidean norm of firing rates appropriately normalized by the number of neurons: (Fig. 5f, and Supplementary Fig. 5d). For our neural recordings (Fig. 5f, far left), we calculated this separately for 10 non-overlapping splits of the data. For our neural networks (Fig. 5f, middle left, middle right, and far right, and Supplementary Fig. 5d), we calculated this separately for 10 different networks. For our optimized neural networks, to emulate alternative tasks where different combinations of color, shape, and width are relevant, we also performed the same analysis when shuffling all 6 input weights (after learning) in all 14 possible ways (Supplementary Fig. 5d, blue lines, ‘shuffle inputs’). These 14 possible shuffles excluded the original set up of input weights, any re-ordering of inputs within a single input channel (since any re-ordering within a single input channel would be identical up to a re-labelling), and any re-ordering between the shape and width input channels (since any re-ordering within the shape and width input channels would also be identical up to a re-labelling).
1.4 Statistics
For decoding analyses, we used non-parametric permutation tests to calculate statistical significance. We used 100 different random shuffles of condition labels across trials to generate a null distribution for decoding accuracy. We plotted chance level decoding (Fig. 3a,b, Supplementary Figs. 1–4, and Supplementary Fig. 5a,b) by combining both early and late learning null distributions.
To calculate significant differences in decoding accuracy over learning (Fig. 3a,b, Supplementary Figs. 1–4, and Supplementary Fig. 5a,b), our test statistic was the difference in decoding accuracy between early and late learning, and our null distribution was the difference in decoding accuracy between early and late learning for the 100 different shuffles of condition labels (see above). We calculated two-tailed p-values for all tests. Additionally, to control for time-related cluster-based errors, we also added a cluster-based permutation correction 70.
For all other tests, we used a two-sided Mann–Whitney U test (Fig. 4h,i, Fig. 5f, and Supplementary Fig. 5d).
Supplementary figurse
References
- 1.Learning shapes neural dimensionality in the prefrontal cortexbioRxiv
- 2.The importance of mixed selectivity in complex cognitive tasksNature 497:585–590
- 3.Task representations in neural networks trained to perform many cognitive tasksNat. Neurosci 22:297–306
- 4.Flexible timing by temporal scaling of cortical responsesNature Neuroscience 21:102–110
- 5.Mastering the game of Go with deep neural networks and tree searchNature 529:484–489
- 6.A recurrent network model of planning explains hippocampal replay and human behaviorbioRxiv
- 7.Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortexScience 291:312–316
- 8.Context-dependent computation by recurrent dynamics in prefrontal cortexNature 503:78–84
- 9.Mixed selectivity morphs population codes in prefrontal cortexNature Neuroscience 20:1770–1779
- 10.The Geometry of Abstraction in the Hippocampus and Prefrontal CortexCell 183:954–967
- 11.Learning shapes cortical dynamics to enhance integration of relevant sensory inputNeuron 111:106–120
- 12.An adaptive coding model of neural function in the prefrontal cortexNature Reviews Neuroscience 2:820–829
- 13.Selective representation of relevant information by neurons in the primate prefrontal cortexNature 393:577–579
- 14.Dynamic coding for cognitive control in prefrontal cortexNeuron 78:364–375
- 15.Orthogonal representations for robust context-dependent task performance in brains and neural networksNeuron 110:1258–1270
- 16.Task switchingTrends in Cognitive Sciences 7:134–140
- 17.The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms frameworkTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16:106–113
- 18.Disentangling with Biological Constraints: A Theory of Functional Cell TypesarXiv
- 19.A neural network that finds a natu-ralistic solution for the production of muscle activityNature Neuroscience 18:1025–1033
- 20.A diverse range of factors affect the nature of neural representations underlying short-term memoryNature Neuroscience 22:275–283
- 21.Regularised neural networks mimic human insightarXiv
- 22.Emergence of grid-like representations by training recurrent neural networks to perform spatial localization6th International Conference on Learning Representations, ICLR 2018 - Conference Track Proceedings :1–19
- 23.A controller-peripheral architecture and costly energy principle for learningbioRxiv
- 24.Optimal anticipatory control as a theory of motor preparation: A thalamo-cortical circuit modelNeuron 109:1567–1581
- 25.Efficient computation and cue integration with noisy population codesNature Neuroscience 4:826–831
- 26.From fixed points to chaos: Three models of delayed discriminationProgress in Neurobiology 103:214–222
- 27.Neuronal Variability: Non-stationary responses to identical visaul stimuliBrain Research 79:405–418
- 28.Energy as a constraint on the coding and processing of sensory informationCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 11:475–480
- 29.Neural Variability in Premotor Cortex Provides a Signature of Motor PreparationJournal of Neuroscience 26:3697–3712
- 30.Metabolic cost as a unifying principle governing neuronal biophysicsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107:12329–12334
- 31.Low-dimensional dynamics for working memory and time encodingProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 117:23021–23032
- 32.Flexible multitask computation in recurrent networks utilizes shared dynamical motifsbioRxiv
- 33.Training Excitatory-Inhibitory Recurrent Neural Networks for Cognitive Tasks: A Simple and Flexible FrameworkPLoS Computational Biology 12:1–30
- 34.Cortical-like dynamics in recurrent circuits optimized for sampling-based probabilistic inferenceNature Neuroscience 23:1138–1149
- 35.Optimal information loading into working memory in prefrontal cortex explains dynamic codingProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 120
- 36.Convolutional neural networks as a model of the visual system: Past, present, and futureJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 33:2017–2031
- 37.No Free Lunch from Deep Learning in Neuroscience: A Case Study through Models of the Entorhinal-Hippocampal CircuitbioRxiv
- 38.Mouse prefrontal cortex represents learned rules for categorizationNature 593:411–417
- 39.Abrupt transitions between prefrontal neural ensemble states accompany behavioral transitions during rule learningNeuron 66:438–448
- 40.The interplay between randomness and structure during learning in RNNsAdvances in Neural Information Processing Systems 33:13352–13362
- 41.Synaptic Transmission Optimization Predicts Expression Loci of Long-Term PlasticityNeuron 96:177–189
- 42.A deep learning framework for neuroscienceNature Neuroscience 22:1761–1770
- 43.Dynamic population coding of category information in inferior temporal and prefrontal cortexJournal of Neurophysiology 100:1407–1419
- 44.Characterizing the dynamics of mental representations: The temporal generalization methodTrends in Cognitive Sciences 18:203–210
- 45.Untangling invariant object recognitionTrends in Cognitive Sciences 11:333–341
- 46.Circuit mechanisms for the maintenance and manipulation of information in working memoryNature Neuroscience 22:1159–1167
- 47.When and why does motor preparation arise in recurrent neural network models of motor control?bioRxiv
- 48.The role of population structure in computations through neural dynamicsNature Neuroscience 25:783–794
- 49.An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex functionAnnual Review of Neuroscience 24:167–202
- 50.Intracellular Determinants of Hippocampal CA1 Place and Silent Cell Activity in a Novel EnvironmentNeuron 70:109–120
- 51.Timecourse of object-related neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex during a short-term memory taskEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 15:1244–1254
- 52.Task-specific neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortexJournal of Neurophysiology 84:451–459
- 53.Filtering of neural signals by focused attention in the monkey prefrontal cortexNature Neuroscience 5:671–676
- 54.Ventro-medial prefrontal cortex compression during concept learningNature Communications 11:1–11
- 55.One-Dimensional Dynamics of Attention and Decision Making in LIPNeuron 58:15–25
- 56.Bayesian Computation through Cortical Latent DynamicsNeuron 103:934–947
- 57.Reservoir Computing Properties of Neural Dynamics in Prefrontal CortexPLoS Computational Biology 12:1–35
- 58.Realtime computing without stable states: A new framework for neural computation based on perturbationsNeural Computation 14:2531–2560
- 59.Why neurons mix: High dimensionality for higher cognitionCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 37:66–74
- 60.Multitasking Capability Versus Learning Efficiency in Neural Network ArchitecturesCogSci 2017 - Proceedings of the 39th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society :829–834
- 61.Linking Connectivity, Dynamics, and Computations in Low-Rank Recurrent Neural NetworksNeuron 99:609–623
- 62.Contextual inference underlies the learning of senso-rimotor repertoiresNature 600:489–493
- 63.Dimensionality, information and learning in prefrontal cortexPLoS Computational Biology 16:1–26
- 64.Principles of neural scienceNew York: McGraw-hill
- 65.Synapses: The Brain’s Energy-Demanding SitesInternational Journal of Molecular Sciences 23
- 66.Synaptic Energy Use and SupplyNeuron 75:762–777
- 67.Energy expenditure computation of a single bursting neuronCognitive Neurodynamics 13:75–87
- 68.Adam: A method for stochastic optimizationarXiv
- 69.A category-free neural population supports evolving demands during decision-makingNature Neuroscience 17:1784–1792
- 70.Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-dataJournal of Neuroscience Methods 164:177–190
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Stroud et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 51
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.