Abstract
The ability to distinguish strangers from familiar individuals is crucial for the survival of most mammalian species. In humans, an inability to recognize kin and familiar individuals and engage in appropriate behaviors is associated with several types of dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease. Mice preferentially spend more time investigating a novel individual relative to a familiar individual. Yet, how social novelty related information drives increased investigation of the novel animal remains poorly understood. Recent evidence has implicated the ventral hippocampus (vHPC) as a key node in encoding information about conspecific identity. Of particular interest are vHPC projections to the lateral septum (LS), a region that has been implicated in driving a wide range of motivated social behaviors. In this study using chemogenetics, optogenetics and monosynaptic rabies tracing, we identified a novel vHPC-LS-ventral tegmental area (VTA) pathway that is necessary for mice to preferentially investigate novel conspecifics. Using monosynaptic rabies tracing, we established that LS neurons make direct monosynaptic connections onto dopaminergic neurons in the VTA. Thus, we have identified a potential pathway via which conspecific identity could be transformed to drive motivated social behaviors.
Introduction
Most animals make daily calculations to approach or avoid a conspecific based on the quality of prior interactions. Yet, how we recognize who we interact with and how that information is transformed to guide subsequent behavior remains poorly understood. For instance, mice readily discriminate between novel and familiar conspecifics, preferentially spending more time investigating a novel individual relative to a familiar individual1. However, it remains unknown how social novelty drives increased approach and investigation.
The ventral hippocampal (vHPC) and dorsal CA2 neurons are known to be causally involved in allowing mice to distinguish between novel and familiar conspecifics2–6. Specifically, studies have shown that manipulating the activity of ventral CA1 but not dorsal CA1 neurons in the hippocampus disrupts the ability of mice to discriminate between novel and familiar conspecifics3. Notably, individual vHPC neurons are known to preferentially fire in the proximity of conspecifics but not objects7. Additionally, experiments have shown that ensembles of neurons in the vHPC encode the identity of a conspecific and that this representation changes as a function of familiarity with the conspecific3. However, it is unclear how this social memory information in the vHPC is transformed to preferentially drive increased investigation of a novel conspecific. Moreover, there remains debate regarding which regions downstream of the vHPC play a role in promoting differential investigation of novel and familiar conspecifics3,4.
This study focused on ventral hippocampal projections to the lateral septum (LS)8. The LS, a subcortical structure primarily composed of GABAergic neurons9–11, receives dense projections from the vHPC and is well situated to orchestrate social novelty-related approach behaviors8,12–16. For instance, LS neurons in rat pups differentially respond to sibling and non-sibling odors and are necessary for the expression of non-sibling preference in older pups17. Also, disrupting vasopressin receptor function in the LS modulates the ability of rodents to discriminate between novel and familiar conspecifics18,19. Most importantly, the LS is thought to directly project to the ventral tegmental area (VTA)20, a region previously implicated in social novelty-related behaviors21–26. Thus, the lateral septum is well suited to act on social memory-related information from the ventral hippocampus to, in turn, regulate motivated social behaviors.
To determine if the vHPC-LS circuit is causally involved in preferentially investigating a novel conspecific over a familiar conspecific, we chemogenetically silenced vHPC-LS neurons and found that mice then spent equal amounts of time investigating a novel and familiar conspecific. Furthermore, we found that optogenetically inhibiting the activity of vHPC-LS neurons in a temporally and spatially specific manner caused the mice to investigate the conspecific paired with the inhibition. We hypothesized that this increased social investigation with vHPC-LS inhibition might arise from disinhibition of the VTA. Using monosynaptic rabies tracing technology, we determined that LS-VTA neurons receive dense ventral hippocampal inputs. Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that chemogenetically silencing LS-VTA neuron activity caused mice to no longer prefer the novel conspecific. Finally, using monosynaptic rabies tracing experiments in Th-Cre+ mice, we found that the LS makes direct monosynaptic connections onto dopaminergic neurons in the VTA. Taken together, we have identified a vHPC-LS-VTA circuitry that appears to play a key role in transforming social memory-related information in the ventral hippocampus to subsequently drive social novelty-related approach behaviors.
Results
Inhibiting vHPC-LS neurons disrupts the preference for social novelty
To determine whether vHPC-LS neurons played a causal role in allowing animals to discriminate between novel and familiar mice, we chemogenetically silenced vHPC-LS neurons while evaluating the effects on behavior in the social discrimination task (SDT). To selectively silence vHPC-LS neurons, we injected C57/BL6 mice in the LS with a retrogradely transporting Cre virus and an AAV5 virus expressing Cre-dependent inhibitory DREADD (hM4Di)27 tagged with mCherry (or a control virus expressing only mCherry) in the vHPC (Figure 1A, B). Three weeks after surgery, we injected mice (i.p.) with either saline or CNO 30 minutes prior to the SDT (Figure 1C). In the SDT, mice were allowed to explore an arena containing age- and sex-matched novel and familiar conspecifics (Figure 1C). Typically, mice spend more time in the proximity of a novel conspecific relative to the familiar conspecific1. We found that chemogenetic inhibition of vHPC-LS neurons by administration of CNO but not saline in hM4Di expressing mice (male and female) disrupted the ability of mice to preferentially spend more time in the proximity of the novel animal (Figure 1F; two-factor ANOVA with drug condition and conspecific identity as factors; interaction: p = 8.4E-04, main effect of conspecific identity: p = 0.003; post hoc Sidak multiple comparison tests: Saline: p = 2E-05, CNO: p = 0.971; discrimination score: one sample t-test, Saline: p = 0.0009, CNO: p = 0.9222; Supplementary figure 1). CNO administration in mCherry expressing control mice did not affect their natural preference for the novel conspecific (Figure 1D; two-factor ANOVA with drug condition and conspecific identity as factors; interaction: p = 0.638, main effect of conspecific identity: p = 6.40E-06; post hoc Sidak multiple comparison tests: Saline: p = 1.10E-04, CNO: p = 1.90E-05). In both saline and CNO conditions, the discrimination scores of the mCherry mice show that mice preferentially spend more time investigating the novel conspecific relative to familiar conspecific (Figure 1E; one sample t-test, Saline: p = 0.0103, CNO: p = 0.0013) while inhibition of vHPC-LS neurons disrupts the preference for the novel mouse (Figure 1G; one sample t-test, Saline: p = 0.0009, CNO: p = 0.9222).
To determine if the effects observed with vHPC-LS inhibition are socially specific, we ran the mice through a food discrimination assay in which mice were allowed to explore an arena containing novel and familiar foods. Unlike with conspecifics, mice are neophobic and do not prefer novel foods28. Therefore, we expected mice would spend less time around the novel food in comparison to the familiar food. All mice exhibited a preference for the familiar food regardless of virus or drug condition (Figure 1H-K; two-factor ANOVA with food identity and drug condition as factors; mCherry: interaction: p = 0.254, main effect of food preference: p = 0.003, post hoc Sidak multiple comparison tests: Saline: p = 0.0323, CNO: p = 9E-04; discrimination score: one sample t-test, Saline: p = 0.0134, CNO: p = 0.008; hM4Di: interaction: p = 0.508, main effect of food preference: p = 3.2E-05, post hoc Sidak multiple comparison tests: Saline: p = 1.7E-04, CNO: p = 0.003; discrimination score: one sample t-test, Saline: p = 0.0016, CNO: p = 0.0419). This finding suggests that the effects observed with vHPC-LS inhibition are socially specific.
To determine if the reduction in preference for a novel conspecific in the SDT could arise from anxiety-like behaviors, we ran mice through an open field assay. We found that chemogenetic silencing of vHPC-LS neurons had no effect on the time spent in the center of the arena (Figure 1L, N; paired t-test, mCherry: p = 0.3167, hM4Di: p = 0.1837). Additionally, chemogenetic inhibition of vHPC-LS neurons had no effect on locomotion, as evidenced by the absence of any changes in the velocity of the animals (Figure 1M, O; paired t-test, mCherry: p = 0.2694, hM4Di: p = 0.7886). Thus, ruling out the possibility that altered locomotion could be contributing to the ability of mice to discriminate between novel and familiar conspecifics is crucial for a comprehensive understanding of their behavior.
Taken together, vHPC-LS chemogenetic inhibition disrupted the ability of mice to preferentially investigate a novel conspecific over a familiar conspecific while not affecting their ability to discriminate between novel and familiar foods. However, it is unclear whether chemogenetic inhibition increases the preference for a familiar mouse or decreases preference for a novel mouse. To distinguish between these possibilities, we next optogenetically silenced the activity of vHPC-LS neurons in a temporally and spatially restricted manner.
vHPC-LS neuron inhibition increases investigation of a mouse paired with inhibition
To determine whether inhibition of vHPC-LS neurons differentially influences the investigation of a novel versus a familiar mouse, we used halorhodopsin (NpHR) to silence vHPC-LS neurons in a temporally and spatially specific manner. We injected wild-type mice with a retrogradely transporting Cre virus in the LS and a Cre-dependent NpHR-EYFP or EGFP-only virus in the vHPC and then implanted fibers in the vHPC to target vHPC-LS cell bodies (Figure 2A, B, Supplementary figure 2). Using a closed-loop optogenetic manipulation (See Methods), we selectively inhibited vHPC-LS neurons when a conspecific entered a previously defined social zone (Figure 2C).
Interestingly, we found that inhibition of vHPC-LS neurons in the proximity of a familiar mouse abolished the natural preference mice have for novel conspecifics (Figure 2F, G; two-factor ANOVA with light condition and conspecific identity as factors; interaction: p = 0.024, main effect of conspecific identity: p = 0.006, Sidak post hoc test; OFF: p = 0.006, N-ON: p = 0.037, F-ON: p = 0.949; discrimination score: one sample t-test, OFF: p = 0.002, NON: p = 0.19, FON: p = 0.719). Significantly, light stimulation did not affect the preference for the novel mouse in the control-EGFP group (Figure 2D, E; two-factor ANOVA with light condition and conspecific identity as factors; interaction: p = 0.898, main effect of conspecific identity: p = 2.8E-05, Sidak post hoc test; OFF: p = 0.032, N-ON: p = 0.012, F-ON: p = 0.032; discrimination score: one sample t-test, OFF: p = 0.008, NON: p = 0.009, FON: p = 0.028). These findings raise the possibility that inhibiting vHPC-LS neurons in the proximity of the familiar mouse could drive increased investigation of that mouse, thus decreasing the time spent in the proximity of a novel mouse.
To determine if silencing of vHPC-LS neurons increased investigation of the mouse paired with stimulation, we inhibited vHPC-LS neurons of test mice in the proximity of one of two novel mice in an arena (Figure 2H-K). We found that NpHR injected mice spent more time in the proximity of the novel mouse that was paired with inhibition than the other novel animal (Figure 2J, K; two-factor ANOVA with light condition and conspecific identity as factors; interaction: p = 0.040, main effect of conspecific identity p = 0.005; post hoc Sidak tests; N-OFF: p = 0.961, N-ON: p = 0.001; discrimination score: one sample t-test, OFF: p = 0.796, NON: p = 0.029). The control-EGFP group showed an equal preference for both novel animals regardless of the light condition (Figure 2H, I; two-factor ANOVA with light condition and conspecific identity as factors; interaction: p = 0.006, no main effect of conspecific identity: p = 0.608; discrimination score: one sample t-test, OFF: p = 0.535, NON: p = 0.128).
Importantly, we found that the increased investigation observed with inhibition of vHPC-LS neurons were socially specific. Inhibition of vHPC-LS activity in the proximity of a novel object did not increase the time spent in the proximity of the object (Figure 2N, O; two-factor ANOVA with light condition and object identity as factors; interaction: p = 0.871, no main effect of conspecific identity: p = 0.505; discrimination score: one sample t-test, OFF: p = 0.720, NON: p = 0.905). Light stimulation also did not change preferences in the control EGFP group when presented with two novel objects (Figure 2L, M; two-factor ANOVA with light condition and object identity as factors; interaction: p = 0.878, no main effect of object identity: p = 0.220; discrimination Score: one sample t-test, OFF: p = 0.697, NON: p = 0.422). These findings suggest that the increased investigation of a mouse paired with vHPC-LS inhibition was not simply a result of the inhibition being appetitive.
To determine if changes in locomotion or anxiety-related behaviors could drive increased investigation of the mouse paired with inhibition, we compared both the time spent in the center and velocity of the animals with and without inhibition of the vHPC-LS neurons while mice explored an open field. We found that inhibition of vHPC-LS neurons did not affect either the velocity or the time spent in the center of the arena in the open field assay (Figure 2R, S; one-way ANOVA, time spent: p = 0.254; velocity: p = 0.775). Light stimulation also had no effect on time spent in the center of the open field arena and the velocity of the animals in the control-EGFP group (Figure 2P, Q; one-way ANOVA, time spent: p = 0.094; velocity: p = 0.259).
Together, these results suggest that vHPC-LS inhibition likely disrupts preference for a novel conspecific by increasing investigation of a familiar conspecific. Thus, demonstrating that intact activity in the vHPC-LS pathway is necessary for mice to engage in social novelty-related behaviors. These findings raise the possibility that inhibition of the vHPC-LS neurons could disinhibit downstream regions that have been implicated in promoting approach of novel conspecifics, like the VTA21–26, by decreasing the inhibitory drive that GABAergic LS projection neurons exert on their downstream targets.
LS-VTA neurons receive dense monosynaptic input from the vHPC
We hypothesized that effects observed with vHPC-LS inhibition on social novelty related approach behaviors could be mediated via LS projections to the VTA. To test this hypothesis, we first had to establish the existence of the vHPC-LS-VTA circuitry. To confirm that LS neurons do project to the VTA, we injected a retrogradely transported Cre virus into the VTA and a Cre-dependent GFP virus into the LS (Figure 3A, B). From dense GFP labeling in the LS, we were able to confirm that LS neurons projected to the VTA (Figure 3C).
Next, we determined if LS-VTA neurons received monosynaptic inputs from the vHPC using a modified monosynaptic rabies tracing technique29. Three weeks after injecting a retrogradely transporting Cre virus in the VTA and a Cre-dependent rabies helper virus in the LS, we injected the modified delta-G deleted rabies virus into the LS (Figure 3D-F). Using the semi-automated WholeBrain software30, we mapped brain wide monosynaptic inputs onto LS-VTA neurons (Figure 3G). We quantified the proportion of total neurons from each region that directly synapse onto LS-VTA neurons. The hippocampal formation was the largest source of inputs to LS-VTA neurons (Figure 3H; one-way ANOVA: p = 0.0107, Post hoc multiple comparison Dunnett’s test; HPF vs IC: p = 0.1579, HPF vs HY: p = 0.0206, HPF vs TH: p = 0.0056). Within the hippocampus, the CA1 and CA3 neurons projected most densely onto LS-VTA neurons (Figure 3I; one-way ANOVA: p = 0.0096, Post hoc multiple comparison Dunnett’s test; CA1 vs DG: p = 0.0411, CA1 vs SUBd: p = 0.0511, CA3 vs DG: p = 0.0134, CA3 vs SUBd: p = 0.0166, CA3 vs SUBv: p = 0.0325.). When looking at the organization of inputs along the dorsoventral axis of the hippocampus, we observed that significantly more vCA1 than dCA1 neurons project to LS-VTA neurons (Figure 3J; paired t-test: p = 0.0496). In contrast, an equivalent proportion of both dCA3 and vCA3 neurons project onto LS-VTA neurons (Figure 3K; paired t-test: p = 0.7736).
From rabies tracing experiments, we have established that the vHPC neurons, specifically neurons from the vCA1 and vCA3 subregions, make dense and direct monosynaptic connections onto LS neurons that in turn project to the VTA. Thus, establishing the existence of a hippocampal-septal-ventral tegmental area circuit that could play a critical role in shaping social novelty discrimination.
LS-VTA neurons play a role in social discrimination and food discrimination
After establishing that LS-VTA neurons do receive monosynaptic inputs from the vHPC, we next asked if LS projections to the VTA played a causal role in allowing mice to preferentially investigate a novel conspecific. To test this possibility, we chemogenetically silenced LS-VTA neurons while mice explored the SDT arena containing a novel and familiar conspecific. We injected a retrogradely transporting Cre virus in the VTA of C57BL/6 mice and a Cre-dependent inhibitory (hM4Di) DREADD virus or an mCherry virus in the LS (Figure 4A, B). Three weeks after surgery, mice received i.p. injections of either saline or CNO to 30 minutes prior to being run on the SDT (Figure 4C).
Interestingly, inhibition of hM4Di expressing LS-VTA neurons with CNO strongly disrupted the ability of mice to preferentially investigate the novel conspecific in the SDT (Figure 4F, G; two-factor ANOVA with drug condition and conspecific identity as factors; interaction: p = 5.1E-04, post hoc Sidak tests: Saline: p = 001, CNO: p = 0.091; discrimination score: one sample t-test, Saline: p = 0.012, CNO: p = 0.208). Importantly, control (mCherry) mice exhibited a strong preference for the novel conspecific over the familiar conspecific in both saline and CNO conditions (Figure 4D, E; two-factor ANOVA with drug condition and conspecific identity as factors; interaction: p = 0.307, main effect of conspecific identity p = 6.8E-04, post hoc Sidak tests; Saline: p = 2.2E-04, CNO: p = 0.0031; discrimination score: one sample t-test, Saline: p = 0.045, CNO: p = 0.032).
Surprisingly, we found that, unlike with vHPC-LS inhibition, inhibiting LS-VTA neurons appears to cause a trend towards disrupting the mice’s normal preference for familiar foods. Inhibiting the activity of LS-VTA neurons caused mice to trend towards spending equal amounts of time in the proximity of novel and familiar foods (Figure 4J, K; two-factor ANOVA with drug condition and food identity as factors; interaction: p = 0.076; discrimination score: one sample t-test, Saline: p = 0.065, CNO: p = 0.863). Control animals preferred the familiar food in both saline and CNO conditions (Figure 4H, I; two-factor ANOVA with drug condition and food identity as factors; interaction: p = 0.941; main effect of food preference: p = 2.7 E-05; discrimination score: one sample t-test, Saline: p = 0.007, CNO: p = 0.055).
Additionally, we were able to confirm that these effects observed with LS-VTA inhibition were not driven by non-specific effects on anxiety-related behaviors. We observed that CNO administration had no effect on the time spent in the center of the open field arena in both control and hM4Di animals (Figure 4L, N; paired t-test, mCherry: p = 0.2196, hM4Di: p = 0.2182). We also observed that CNO administration did not affect speed of the animals in both control and hM4Di animals (Figure 4M, O; paired t-test, mCherry: p = 0.0733, NpHR: p = 0.7193).
These findings provide compelling evidence that the increased investigation observed with vHPC-LS inhibition is mediated via its projection onto the VTA. Dopamine neurons in the VTA show increased activity while mice investigate a novel conspecific and are known to be involved in causally driving increased investigation of a novel conspecific21–26. Together, these findings raise the intriguing possibility that silencing vHPC-LS neurons could disinhibit dopamine neurons in the VTA, which, in turn, could drive increased approach and investigation of a conspecific. Although we have established LS projects to the VTA, it is unclear if LS projects to the dopaminergic neurons in the VTA.
LS neurons synapse directly onto dopamine neurons in the VTA
To determine if LS neurons synapse directly onto dopamine neurons in the VTA, we mapped monosynaptic inputs onto dopaminergic neurons in the VTA ( VTADA) by applying the modified rabies tracing method in Th-Cre+ mice. First, we injected a Cre-dependent helper AAV virus into the VTA of Th-Cre+ mice. After a three week period, we injected a deltaG deleted rabies virus expressing mCherry into the VTA (Figure 5A, B). Using the semi-automated WholeBrain software30, we mapped and registered mCherry labeled cells along the entire anterior-posterior axis of the LS (Figure 5C). These mCherry labeled cells signify LS neurons that make monosynaptic connections onto VTADA neurons. We next quantified the anatomical distribution of VTADA projecting LS neurons in the brain (Figure 5D-F). Amongst the various subdivisions within the LS, we found that the rostral subdivision of the LS (LSr) projected most strongly to the VTA (Figure 5G; one-way ANOVA: p = 5E-06, post hoc Tukey test; LSr vs LSv: p = 1.2E-05, LSr vs LSv: p = 6E-06, LSc vs LSv: p = 0.26). Thus, we have identified a novel pathway that connects LS neurons directly to dopaminergic VTA neurons that could serve as the neural substrate to drive social novelty related behaviors.
Discussion
Here, we show that chemogenetically and optogenetically silencing vHPC-LS neurons disrupts the ability of mice to preferentially investigate a novel conspecific. Furthermore, closed-loop, spatially constrained optogenetic inhibition of vHPC-LS neurons increased investigation of the mouse paired with inhibition. These findings led us to hypothesize that inhibiting vHPC-LS neurons could increase preference for a conspecific by disinhibiting the VTA, a region heavily implicated in social novelty and social approach behaviors. Consistent with this hypothesis, through monosynaptic rabies tracing experiments, we found that vHPC-LS neurons project heavily to the VTA. We next discovered that chemogenetic silencing of LS-VTA neurons also disrupted the ability of mice to preferentially engage with a novel conspecific. Finally, using monosynaptic rabies tracing technology in Th-Cre+ mice, we found that dopaminergic neurons in the VTA receive direct monosynaptic inputs from the LS. Given that LS neurons synapse directly onto VTA dopamine neurons, inhibiting vHPC-LS neurons could disinhibit VTA dopamine neurons, thus driving increased social approach and investigation. We have identified a circuit necessary for mice to differentially investigate novel and familiar conspecifics.
Rodents, including mice, behave differently around novel and familiar conspecifics31,32. When given a choice, mice preferentially spend more time near a novel mouse than a familiar mouse. In rodents, optogenetic and imaging experiments have identified the vHPC to be causally involved in distinguishing between familiar and novel conspecifics. However, how information about the conspecific identity thought to be primarily encoded in the vHPC3 is transformed to drive increased approach and investigation of a novel individual remains poorly understood. In particular, it remains unclear which structures downstream of the hippocampus play a role in regulating differential investigation of a novel over a familiar conspecific. The vHPC sends dense projections onto the nucleus accumbens (NAc), medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the LS12. Recent studies have suggested a key role for hippocampal projections to the mPFC and the NAc in discriminating between novel and familiar conspecifics3,4. However, some questions remain. While optogenetic inhibition of vHPC-NAc neurons disrupted the ability of mice to discriminate novel and familiar conspecifics3, chemogenetic manipulation of vHPC-NAc neurons did not affect preference for a novel conspecific4. Moreover, while chemogenetic manipulation of vHPC-mPFC activity disrupted the formation of social memory, its effects on modulating social preference for the novel mouse during acute recall in wildtype mice remains less obvious4. Thus, raising the possibility that an alternative pathway from the vHPC could help mice engage in differential social behavior when presented with a novel and familiar conspecific. In our study, using optogenetic and chemogenetic manipulations, we have found that the vHPC-LS pathway plays a key role in modulating preference for a novel conspecific.
vCA1 neurons respond robustly in the proximity of conspecifics and subsets of vCA1 neurons are known to preferentially respond in the proximity of familiar conspecifics3,6,7. Additionally, a larger fraction of vCA1 neurons respond in the proximity of a familiar conspecific compared to a novel conspecific in a social discrimination assay3. Thus, raising the possibility that lowering the levels of vCA1 neuron activity could create a perception of social novelty and could result in increased investigation of a mouse. Consistent with this idea, we found that optogenetically silencing vHPC-LS neurons increased investigation of the mouse paired with investigation. Findings from our rabies tracing findings show that both vCA1 and vCA3 neurons project to the LS (Figure 3J, K). The viral injections in our study were set up to target the vHPC broadly, labeling both vCA1 and vCA3 neurons projecting to the LS. Therefore, the behavioral effects observed with the chemogenetic and optogenetic experiments cannot be localized to only the vCA1 or the vCA3 neurons specifically. A recent study showed that vCA1 and vCA3 neurons differentially modulate approach avoidance conflict task33. While silencing vCA1 neurons induced increased avoidance of a conflict cue, silencing vCA1 neurons resulted in increased approach of the conflict cue. Additionally, recent data suggests that vCA1 and vCA3 neurons form parallel septal pathways that play differential roles in modulating approach avoidance behaviors in response to conflict cues34. Given that vHPC-LS inhibition did not have any effect on object (Figure 2N, O) or food preference (Figure 1J, K) in our study, it is unclear if the same vHPC-LS circuits involved in conflict approach/avoidance are also involved in mediating the preference and approach elicited by a novel conspecific. Thus, future studies specifically targeting and manipulating only vCA1 or vCA3 inputs to the LS are necessary to determine if these parallel pathways play differential roles in mediating preferential investigation of a novel conspecific over a familiar conspecific.
The LS, with connections to downstream regions that are heavily implicated in mediating social behaviors, like the NAc35–40, VTA21–26,41,42 and ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus (vmH)8,43–47, is well positioned to translate the social recognition related information it receives from the vHPC into motivated behaviors. In this study, we focused on the LS projections to the VTA, a region that has been shown to play a key role in modulating social novelty. Dopamine neurons in the VTA have been shown to be differentially modulated by novel and familiar conspecifics22,23,48–50. Chemogenetic inhibition of dopamine neurons in the VTA attenuates investigation of a novel conspecific24. We therefore hypothesized that the LS could regulate social novelty preference by modulating the activity of dopamine neurons in the VTA. Using transsynaptic rabies tracing, we identified direct monosynaptic inputs from the LS onto dopamine neurons in the VTA (Figure 5). Importantly, we found that inhibiting LS-VTA neurons disrupted the ability of mice to preferentially investigate a novel conspecific.
Unlike the socially specific effects observed with vHPC-LS inhibition, LS-VTA inhibition also disrupted the natural preference that mice exhibit towards investigating familiar foods (Figure 4J, K). These findings suggest that LS-VTA neurons are more broadly involved in mediating preference for a variety of ethologically relevant stimuli. Consistent with this idea, VTA dopamine neurons are known to play a key role in mediating the motivation to seek both food rewards and social investigation22–24,51–54. Additionally, a large proportion of individual VTADA neurons respond to both food and social stimuli55. VTADA neurons are active during both the consumption of familiar palatable food and during increased investigation of a novel conspecific22–24,51–54. Thus, inhibiting LS neurons that project to the VTA (including VTADA neurons) could disrupt both social and food seeking behaviors.
Although our data support the view that inhibition of LS-VTA neurons could be exerting an effect on social novelty preference behaviors by directly modulating the activity of dopamine neurons in the VTA, it remains unknown if LS-VTA neurons also act on GABAergic neurons in the VTA. Local infusions of GABA into the caudodorsal LS was shown to both increase and decrease the responses of VTA neurons in response to stimulation of vCA3 neurons, raising the possibility that LS neurons could project to GABAergic neurons in the VTA20. However, it remains to be determined if GABAergic neurons in the VTA receive direct monosynaptic input from the LS. Additionally, future recording and imaging experiments are needed to determine what information LS-VTA neurons encode. For instance, sibling and non-sibling odor representations are anatomically organized along the dorsoventral axis of the LS neurons in rat pups and lesions to the LS disrupt the ability of rat pups to distinguish between kin and non-kin odors17. These findings raise two interesting questions. First, are social familiarity and novelty information differentially represented in the dorsal and ventral populations of LS neurons? Second, are the dorsally located LS-VTA neurons (Figure 3C) necessary for kin recognition? Addressing these questions might provide insight into whether different kinds of recognition information (kin/non-kin, novel/familiar) and context information are integrated and transformed into approach and avoidance behaviors at the level of the LS.
In line with the above ideas, neurons from other hippocampal compartments, besides vCA1 and vCA3. including the dCA3 (Figure 3K) and dCA256,57 project to the lateral septum. These hippocampal projections have also been implicated in mediating social novelty behaviors. For example, activating dCA3-LS neurons was sufficient to restore social novelty preference deficits induced by chronic social defeat stress58. Interestingly, another study found that silencing the vCA3 neurons but not dCA3 neurons disrupted the ability of mice to differentially investigate familiar versus novel animals in control mice (not defeated/stressed)59. This raises the intriguing possibility that, first, social recognition information is represented in a distributed fashion across the various hippocampal compartments and that parallel hippocampal-septal pathways are differentially recruited in a context and internal state-dependent fashion to transform social novelty/ familiarity information into decisions to approach or avoid a conspecific.
Mounting evidence suggests a key role for the lateral septum in modulating a broad range of motivated social behaviors. In addition to the role we are suggesting for the vCA1-LS pathway in modulating social novelty preference, a recent study demonstrated that corticotropin-releasing hormone released by infralimbic (IL) neurons projecting to the rostral LS acts to suppress investigation of a familiar conspecific, thus promoting a relative increase in investigation of a novel conspecific60. Beyond social novelty/familiarity behaviors, the lateral septum plays a key role in modulating aggression56,61–63 and in modulating social reward seeking64. Dorsal LS neurons are known to inhibit ventral LS neurons via local collaterals56,61. Based on this evidence, it has been hypothesized that lateral inhibition between various LS populations could allow for selecting one behavioral output while suppressing the other13,14. For example, increased activity in the ventral LS could suppress dorsal LS and disinhibit regions downstream of the dorsal LS, such as the NAc and the VTA thus promoting increased social approach and investigation while simultaneously suppressing aggression56. Thus, providing an exciting framework in which the LS could play a key role in regulating mutually exclusive social behaviors13,14.
Although we focused largely on social novelty seeking behavior, there is evidence that the hippocampal-septal circuitry plays a role in context-dependent modulation of behavior in a variety of contexts65–67. Although, we saw no effects in the ability of the mice to discriminate between novel and familiar foods with vHPC-LS inhibition in a discrimination assay in which the mice could smell but not access the food, a study found that activation of glutamatergic vHPC-LS neurons suppressed food intake in the home cage68. Additionally, a recent study suggests that vHPC-LS neurons are necessary for mice to learn food reward - location associations67. It is unclear if this finding extends to socially rewarding stimuli. It also remains to be seen if the same vHPC-LS neurons/subcircuits that are involved in mediating social preference also modulate other motivated behaviors.
In summary, we identify a novel vHPC-LS-VTA pathway that regulates social novelty preference. Our study provides insight into how social memory related information is transformed into motivated social behaviors. Our study lays the groundwork for future experiments that could help us understand how these circuits might be disrupted in neurodegenerative disorders, like Alzheimer’s disease, in which the ability of individuals to recognize and engage in appropriate social behaviors is dramatically affected.
Supplementary figures
Methods
Experimental Model and Subject Details
All experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the National Institutes of Health and Emory University’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC). All behavioral experiments were performed on male and female C57BL/6J (Jax strain Number: 000664) mice aged ∼4-12 weeks. Rabies tracing experiments included in Figures 3 and 5 were performed on male mice with the animals in Figure 5 belonging to the transgenic strains TH-Cre+ (Jax strain Number: 008601). Mice were pair housed or group housed in cages with ad libitum access to water and chow (LabDiet). Mice were maintained on a 12 hour reverse light cycle. All experiments were conducted during their light-off period. In social discrimination tests, stimulus mice were sex and age matched to the test animal.
Stereotactic Surgeries
Mice were placed in a stereotaxic setup (Kopf) and anesthetized with 1-2% isoflurane during surgery. All coordinates were sourced from the Paxinos Atlas69 and are described relative to bregma. Injections were performed using a Nanoject III (Drummond Scientific) and the virus was delivered at a rate of 2nL per second. Virus injection coordinates are as follows, vHPC: 3 mm posterior, 3.25 mm lateral and −4.2 mm ventral relative to bregma and LS: 0.4 mm anterior, 0 mm lateral, −2.8 mm ventral relative to bregma, VTA: 3.1 mm posterior, 0.35 mm lateral, −4.7 mm ventral.
DREADDs
For vHPC-LS chemogenetic silencing experiments, we bilaterally injected RetroAAV-hSyn-Cre (500nL, Addgene Lot v70508, 3*1013) into the LS. This was followed by 250 nl injections of either AAV5-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry or AAV5-hSyn-DIO-mCherry into the vHPC of mice aged 5-6 weeks.
For LS-VTA chemogenetic silencing experiments, we bilaterally injected RetroAAV-hSyn-Cre (500nL) into the VTA. This was followed by 250 nl injections of either AAV5-hSyn-DIO-mCherry or AAV5-hSyn-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry into the LS of mice aged 5-6 weeks.
Optogenetics
For vHPC-LS cell body inhibition experiments, we bilaterally injected with RetroAAV-hSyn-Cre (500nL, Addgene Lot v70508, 3*1013) into the LS. This was followed by 500 nL injections of either AAV5-Ef1-DIO-eNpHR3.0-EGFP (Addgene v32533,1.1*1013) or AAV5-hSyn-DIO-EGFP (Addgene, 1.1*1013) into the vHPC of mice aged 5-6 weeks. Optical fibers attached to ferrules were secured bilaterally using Metabond and dental acrylic to the skull to target the vHPC (Ferrule coordinates: −3.250 anterior, +/−3.0 lateral, −4.0 depth). Animals were allowed to recover and express the virus for 3 weeks prior to behavioral experiments.
Retrograde tracing
To determine if LS neurons project to the VTA, we injected 750 nl of the retroAAV-Ef1a-mCherry-IRES-Cre virus into the VTA and 750 nl of the AAV5-CAG-FLEX-EGFP into the LS. Animals were allowed to recover and express the virus for 3 weeks prior to histology.
Rabies Tracing
For the LS-VTA monosynaptic rabies tracing experiments, 750 nl of the retroAAV-hsyn-Cre (500nL, Addgene Lot v70508, 3*1013) was injected in the VTA of male and female C57/BL6 mice. This was combined with the injection of 750 nl of the helper virus, AAV1.synP.FLEX.splitTVA.EGFP.B19G (Addgene, 2.4*1013) in the LS. Three weeks following injection, all mice were injected with 750 nl of the N2c-ΔG-deleted rabies virus (5*108, Thomas Jefferson University)70 expressing mCherry and pseudotyped with EnvA, RVdG-mCherry, into the LS.
For VTADA monosynaptic rabies tracing experiments, 750 nl of the rabies helper virus, AAV1.synP.FLEX.splitTVA.EGFP.B19G (Addgene, 2.4*1013) was injected into the VTA of Th-Cre+ mice (Jax strain number: 008601). 3 weeks following injection, all mice were injected with 750 nl of the N2c-ΔG-deleted rabies virus expressing mCherry and pseudotyped with EnvA, RVdG-mCherry into the VTA (∼5×108, Thomas Jefferson University).
Viral reagents
Histology and Imaging
Mice were anesthetized with Euthasol (0.1 mg/kg) then transcardially perfused using PBS (0.5X) followed by 4% PFA. Brains were extracted, left in PFA overnight, then transferred to a 30% sucrose solution the following day. A microtome (Leica Biosystems) was used to slice the coronal sections (50 um) and validate viral targeting. Slices were mounted using DAPI Fluoromount (Southern Biotech). All mounted sections were imaged using a high throughput automated stitching fluorescent microscope (Keyence BZ-X810). Native fluorescence of expressed fluorophores was used to confirm expression.
For the chemogenetic silencing experiments, coronal sections of LS (LS-VTA experiments - Figure 4) and coronal sections of vHPC (vHPC-LS experiments - Figure 1) were used to confirm virus expression. 6 mice were excluded from the vHPC-LS cohort for showing no expression or off-targeting. 7 mice were excluded from the LS-VTA cohort for showing no expression or off-targeting.
For cell body optogenetic inhibition experiments (Figure 2), coronal sections of vHPC were used to confirm virus expression, nuclear exclusion of NpHR, and to verify targeting of the fibers to vHPC (Supp. Figure 2). 4 mice were excluded from the vHPC-LS optogenetic cohort for showing no expression or off-targeting.
For monosynaptic rabies tracing experiments (Figure 3, 5), brain sections ranging from the entire brain (∼3.5 mm anterior bregma to ∼4.5 mm posterior bregma) in ∼100um spacing were mounted. Sections were coverslipped with a DAPI Fluoromount mounting media and imaged using an automated widefield whole slide scanner (Keyence BZ810). Input neurons were mCherry labeled and starter cells expressed both EYFP (helper virus) and mCherry (rabies virus) (Figure 3, 5).
Behavioral Assays
Social Discrimination Task (SDT)
All mice used in this assay were ∼ 8 weeks old. The social discrimination chamber (58.42 cm x 25.4 cm x 22.86 cm) contains two cages housing age and sex-matched stimulus mice (cage diameter = 7.62 cm) at opposite ends of the chamber. Prior to being run on the social discrimination assay, the test mouse was co-housed with a same sex, age-matched conspecific for 72 hours to familiarize. The test mice were then allowed to explore the chamber containing both a novel and familiar stimulus mouse for 5 minutes. The location of the novel and familiar mice (left and versus right) was counterbalanced to eliminate side preferences. Behavior was tracked using an overhead camera and the position of the mouse in the arena was tracked using the open-source software Bonsai71. Percent time spent was calculated by measuring the amount of time the test mouse spent around each encaged social target (diameter of the social zone - 60 mm) relative to the entire assay duration.
For chemogenetic experiments (Figure 1, 4), ∼ 3−4 weeks after viral injection, mice expressing either the inhibitory DREADDs or mCherry received intraperitoneal injections of either Clozapine-N-Oxide (HelloBio, 1 mg/kg, i.p.) or saline (0.2 mL, i.p.) 30 minutes prior to being run on the behavioral assay. Mice were not given more than one CNO injection per day.
For vHPC-LS cell body optogenetic inhibition experiments (Figure 2), we used the Bonsai software to track the position of the test mouse in real-time and used this information to specifically inhibit the activity of vHPC-LS neurons only in the proximity of a particular conspecific (social zone diameter: 60mm). The real-time tracking data from Bonsai was used to gate a green laser source via a TTL driver (Pulse Pal, Sanworks). Thus, the green laser source connected to the implanted fibers (532 nm, 6 mW, constant light - at the fiber tip) was turned on when the animal moved into only one of the social zones (either novel or familiar) and remained on until the animal moved out of the zone. The other social zone remained as a control zone and was not paired with light. Time spent by the mice in each zone was later estimated as described above.
For the novel versus familiar comparisons, both NpHR and EGFP mice were run on this assay across several conditions. Condition 1 - control condition-no stimulation paired with either the novel or the familiar mouse. Condition 2 - stimulation around the novel but not familiar animals. Condition 3 - stimulation around the familiar but not novel animal.
The same NpHR and EGFP mice were also run through a novel vs novel mouse experiment. In this experiment, the zone around one of the two novel mice was randomly designated as the stimulation zone, and entry into this zone was paired with green light illumination (light conditions same as above). Time spent by the mice in each of the two zones was estimated using Bonsai.
Food Discrimination Task
Arena and handling procedures are identical to SDT. Familiar food was the chow (3g, LabDiet), while the novel food was a 3g of fruit-flavored cereal. Food was placed under wired cages, such that mice could sniff but not taste the food. Mice were given 5 minutes to explore the arena containing the novel and familiar food. Percent time spent around the novel and familiar food was calculated by measuring the amount of time the test mouse spent around the cage containing the food (diameter of food zone - 6 cm) relative to the entire assay duration.
Object Discrimination Task
Arena and handling procedures are identical to SDT. An object was placed into the home cage of test mice for 72 hours of familiarization. The objects were placed on the opposite ends of the discrimination chamber (without wired cages), and mice were given 5 minutes to explore the novel and familiar objects. For chemogenetic manipulation, procedures were performed identically to the SDT. Novel and familiar objects were placed on either the left or the right side of the chamber in a counterbalanced fashion.
For spatially-restricted optogenetic manipulation, mice were allowed to investigate two novel objects in an arena for 5 minutes. In the light ON condition, one of the two objects picked randomly was paired with green light stimulation (532nm, 6mW at the fiber tip constant light stimulation). Behavior recording conditions and laser conditions were identical to SDT.
Open Field
Mice were allowed to freely explore a large square chamber (46.64 cm x 46.64 cm) for 10 minutes. We determined the amount of time mice spent in the middle of the arena (11.14 cm x 11.14 cm) using Bonsai and MATLAB. For chemogenetic experiments, mice were injected with either saline or CNO 30 minutes prior to the test. For the vHPC-LS cell body inhibition experiment, the open field task was 9 minutes long. Mice performed the assay for 3 minutes with no light stimulation, followed by 3 minutes of green light stimulation (532 nm, 6 mW at the tip of the optical fiber; constant light illumination) and terminated with an additional 3 minutes of no light.
Analyses
Discrimination Score
The discrimination score was calculated by the below equation. Time spent in proximity to novel minus (NTime Spent) time spent in proximity to familiar (FTime Spent), divided by the total duration of time spent in both zones (TotalTime Spent).
Therefore, positive discrimination scores equate a mouse’s preference for novel conspecifics and novel food odors and negative scores indicate an animal’s preference for the familiar conspecific and food odors.
Velocity
The velocity of animals within the Open Field task was determined by calculating distance traveled per unit of time and centroid positioning using Bonsai, an open source tracking software71. Velocity was determined by change in centimeters moved over change in seconds.
Whole Brain
The open source WholeBrain software package in R allows for the annotation, analysis, and visualization of cellular resolution tracing in an interactive brain atlas30. The Whole Brain software was used specifically to register individual brain sections from animals onto the Allen common coordinate framework. It was used to map input neurons in rabies tracing experiments (Figure 3, 5).
Clozapine-N-Oxide
In the projection-specific inactivation experiments, all mice received CNO (Sigma; 1 mg/kg, i.p., in 2% DMSO and saline, 1 ml/ 100 g), regardless of virus condition, to equally expose animals to any unintended consequences of CNO. CNO was always administered 30 min prior to the behavioral experiment.
Statistics
Analyses were performed using PRISM GraphPad, MATLAB, and RStudio. One-way ANOVA or unpaired t-tests were used to compare discrimination scores and velocity. Two-factor ANOVAs were used for social discrimination scores and open field analyses. In the case of unequal variances, Welch’s ANOVA was used. Tukey’s post hoc tests or paired t-tests were used in the case of significant interactions or main effects with >2 groups and are indicated in the figure with p-value annotations being * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Unless otherwise indicated, all tests are two-tailed.
References
- 1.Sociability and preference for social novelty in five inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like behavior in miceGenes Brain Behav 3:287–302
- 2.The hippocampal CA2 region is essential for social memoryNature 508:88–92
- 3.Ventral CA1 neurons store social memoryScience 353:1536–1541
- 4.Ventral hippocampal projections to the medial prefrontal cortex regulate social memoryElife 8
- 5.Coding of social novelty in the hippocampal CA2 region and its disruption and rescue in a 22q11.2 microdeletion mouse modelNat. Neurosci 23:1365–1375
- 6.Disrupted social memory ensembles in the ventral hippocampus underlie social amnesia in autism-associated Shank3 mutant miceMol. Psychiatry 27:2095–2105
- 7.Neuronal Responses to Conspecifics in the Ventral CA1Cell Rep 27:3460–3472
- 8.Connections of the rat lateral septal complexBrain Res. Brain Res. Rev 24:115–195
- 9.Chemoarchitecture of the rat lateral septal nucleus. Brain Res. Rev. 24, 91–113 (1997).Brain Res. Rev 24:91–113
- 10.Multimodal classification of neurons in the lateral septumbioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.02.15.580381
- 11.Opioid-driven disruption of the septal complex reveals a role for neurotensin-expressing neurons in withdrawalbioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.01.15.575766
- 12.Circuit and molecular architecture of a ventral hippocampal networkNat. Neurosci 23:1444–1452
- 13.Top-down regulation of motivated behaviors via lateral septum sub-circuitsMol. Psychiatry 27:3119–3128
- 14.Putting Together Pieces of the Lateral Septum: Multifaceted Functions and Its Neural PathwayseNeuro 8
- 15.Lateral septum as a nexus for mood, motivation, and movementNeurosci. Biobehav. Rev 126:544–559
- 16.Neurobiology of the lateral septum: regulation of social behaviorTrends Neurosci 45:27–40
- 17.The lateral septum mediates kinship behavior in the ratNat. Commun 11
- 18.The V1a vasopressin receptor is necessary and sufficient for normal social recognition: a gene replacement studyNeuron 47:503–513
- 19.Vasopressin infusion into the lateral septum of adult male rats rescues progesterone-induced impairment in social recognitionNeuroscience 246
- 20.Linking context with reward: a functional circuit from hippocampal CA3 to ventral tegmental areaScience 333:353–357
- 21.Involvement of the oxytocin system in the nucleus accumbens in the regulation of juvenile social novelty-seeking behaviorHorm. Behav 93:94–98
- 22.VTA dopamine neuron activity encodes social interaction and promotes reinforcement learning through social prediction errorNat. Neurosci 25
- 23.Natural Neural Projection Dynamics Underlying Social Behavior. Cell 157, 1535–1551 (2014).Cell 157:1535–1551
- 24.Role of VTA dopamine neurons and neuroligin 3 in sociability traits related to nonfamiliar conspecific interactionNat. Commun 9
- 25.Adolescent sleep shapes social novelty preference in miceNat. Neurosci 25
- 26.Reduction in the activity of VTA/SNc dopaminergic neurons underlies aging-related decline in novelty seekingCommunications Biology 6:1–16
- 27.Evolving the lock to fit the key to create a family of G protein-coupled receptors potently activated by an inert ligandProc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 104:5163–5168
- 28.Factors controlling measures of anxiety and responses to novelty in the mouseBehav. Brain Res 125:151–157
- 29.Monosynaptic restriction of transsynaptic tracing from single, genetically targeted neuronsNeuron 53
- 30.An interactive framework for whole-brain maps at cellular resolutionNat. Neurosci 21:139–149
- 31.Social recognition in laboratory mice requires integration of behaviorally-induced somatosensory, auditory and olfactory cuesPsychoneuroendocrinology 143
- 32.Mouse social recognition and preferenceCurr. Protoc. Neurosci 8
- 33.Ventral Hippocampal CA1 and CA3 Differentially Mediate Learned Approach-Avoidance Conflict ProcessingCurr. Biol 28
- 34.Parallel ventral hippocampus-lateral septum pathways differentially regulate approach-avoidance conflictNat. Commun 13
- 35.Responses and functions of dopamine in nucleus accumbens core during social behaviorsCell Rep 40
- 36.Social approach and social vigilance are differentially regulated by oxytocin receptors in the nucleus accumbensNeuropsychopharmacology 45:1423–1430
- 37.Balance Between Projecting Neuronal Populations of the Nucleus Accumbens Controls Social Behavior in MiceBiol. Psychiatry 95:123–135
- 38.Social isolation impairs the prefrontal-nucleus accumbens circuit subserving social recognition in miceCell Rep 35
- 39.Social reward requires coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens oxytocin and serotoninNature 501:179–184
- 40.5-HT release in nucleus accumbens rescues social deficits in mouse autism modelNature 560:589–594
- 41.Gating of social reward by oxytocin in the ventral tegmental areaScience https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aan4994
- 42.The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesisJ. Comp. Neurol 519:3599–3639
- 43.Hypothalamic control of innate social behaviorsScience https://doi.org/10.1126/science.adh8489
- 44.Functional identification of an aggression locus in the mouse hypothalamusNature 470:221–226
- 45.Hierarchical Representations of Aggression in a Hypothalamic-Midbrain CircuitNeuron 106
- 46.Scalable control of mounting and attack by Esr1+ neurons in the ventromedial hypothalamusNature 509:627–632
- 47.Esr1+ cells in the ventromedial hypothalamus control female aggressionNat. Neurosci 20:1580–1590
- 48.A circuit-based mechanism underlying familiarity signaling and the preference for noveltyNat. Neurosci 20
- 49.Midbrain circuits of novelty processingNeurobiol. Learn. Mem 176
- 50.Striatal dopamine explains novelty-induced behavioral dynamics and individual variability in threat predictionNeuron 110
- 51.A neural substrate of prediction and rewardScience 275
- 52.Overlapping representations of food and social stimuli in mouse VTA dopamine neuronsNeuron 111:3541–3553
- 53.Neuron-type-specific signals for reward and punishment in the ventral tegmental areaNature 482:85–88
- 54.High-fat food biases hypothalamic and mesolimbic expression of consummatory drivesNat. Neurosci 23:1253–1266
- 55.Overlapping representations of food and social stimuli in VTA dopamine neuronsbioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.05.17.541104
- 56.A hippocampal CA2 to lateral septal circuit disinhibits social aggressionNature 564
- 57.Lateral septum modulates cortical state to tune responsivity to threat stimuliCell Rep 41
- 58.A circuit from dorsal hippocampal CA3 to parvafox nucleus mediates chronic social defeat stress–induced deficits in preference for social noveltyScience Advances https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abe8828
- 59.A role for CA3 in social recognition memoryBehav. Brain Res 354:22–30
- 60.Corticotropin-releasing hormone signaling from prefrontal cortex to lateral septum suppresses interaction with familiar miceCell 186
- 61.Effective Modulation of Male Aggression through Lateral Septum to Medial Hypothalamus ProjectionCurr. Biol 26
- 62.Neural dynamics in the limbic system during male social behaviorsNeuron 111:3288–3306
- 63.The septal forebrain and the inhibitory modulation of attack and defense in the rat. A reviewBehav. Neural Biol 30:357–388
- 64.Social trauma engages lateral septum circuitry to occlude social rewardNature 613:696–703
- 65.Dorsolateral septum somatostatin interneurons gate mobility to calibrate context-specific behavioral fear responsesNat. Neurosci 22
- 66.Distinct Dorsal and Ventral Hippocampal CA3 Outputs Govern Contextual Fear DiscriminationCell Rep 30
- 67.Ventral hippocampus-lateral septum circuitry promotes foraging-related memoryCell Rep 40
- 68.An excitatory ventral hippocampus to lateral septum circuit that suppresses feedingNat. Commun 6
- 69.Paxinos and Franklin’s the Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic CoordinatesAcademic Press
- 70.Rabies Virus CVS-N2c ΔG Strain Enhances Retrograde Synaptic Transfer and Neuronal ViabilityNeuron 89:711–724
- 71.Bonsai: an event-based framework for processing and controlling data streamsFront. Neuroinform 9
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Rashid et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 795
- downloads
- 62
- citations
- 2
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.