Recruitment of the default mode network during a demanding act of executive control
Abstract
In the human brain, a default mode or task-negative network shows reduced activity during many cognitive tasks and is often associated with internally-directed processes, such as mind wandering and thoughts about the self. In contrast to this task-negative pattern, we show increased activity during a large and demanding switch in task set. Furthermore, we employ multivoxel pattern analysis and find that regions of interest within default mode network are encoding task-relevant information during task performance. Activity in this network may be driven by major revisions of cognitive context, whether internally or externally focused.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.06481.001eLife digest
The default mode network is a network in the brain that is often active when we think about ourselves, reminiscence about the past or just let our minds wander. However, this network—which involves many different regions of the brain—usually becomes inactive when we focus on a specific cognitive task.
Now Crittenden et al. have used a technique called functional MRI to show that the default mode network can become active again if we switch from one task to another. Functional MRI works by measuring the blood flow in the brain: regions of the brain that are active have more blood flow than regions that are not active.
Crittenden et al. studied the brains of human subjects as they performed a series of different tasks. These experiments showed that the activity of the default mode network does not change when the subject is focused on a single task. This is also true for when the subject switches between two similar tasks. However, when the subject switches between two very different tasks, the network becomes significantly more active. Moreover, the patterns of activity in the network seem to reflect the nature of the tasks.
The work of Crittenden et al. strongly suggests that in order to successfully switch between two different tasks, the brain needs to engage the default mode network and allow the mind to wander. Future studies will involve exploring how different the two tasks need to be in order to activate the default mode network, and studying how brain damage within the network may impair patients ability to switch between different tasks.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.06481.002Introduction
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has repeatedly demonstrated that cognitive tasks of many kinds decrease activity in a large-scale cortical network, variously termed the task-negative or default mode network (DMN) (Shulman et al., 1997; Raichle et al., 2001; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010). The DMN consistently shows reduced activity during task performance compared to rest (Raichle and Snyder, 2007) and often reduced activity for harder compared to easier task versions (Gilbert et al., 2012). In contrast to this general pattern, increased activity has been reported in a cluster of mental states involving thinking about the self, one's own perspective, or the perspective of others (Buckner and Carroll, 2007). Examples include recollecting previous experiences (Vincent et al., 2006) or imagining future ones (Addis et al., 2007), mind-wandering (Mason et al., 2007), and theory of mind tasks (Young et al., 2010). The DMN has thus been linked to a number of high-level cognitive processes, such as self-referential processing (Gusnard et al., 2001) and imaginary scene construction (Hassabis and Maguire, 2007).
Recently, Andrews-Hanna et al. (2010) have argued that the DMN separates into three sub-networks. Using graph theoretical analytic approaches to resting-state fRMI data, Andrews-Hanna et al. identified a core sub-network comprising bilateral posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and anterior medial prefrontal cortex (AMPFC) a medial temporal lobe (MTL) sub-network made up of ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), bilateral hippocampal formation (HF), parahippocampus (PHC), retrosplenial cortex (Rsp), and posterior inferior parietal lobule (pIPL) and a dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) sub-network which includes the DMPFC, bilateral temporal parietal junction (TPJ), lateral temporal cortex (LTC), and the temporal Pole (TempP). Andrews-Hanna et al. argue for a degree of functional segregation between these sub-networks, with the MTL sub-network especially linked to construction of mental scenes based on memory, and the DMPFC network more involved in mentalising. These segregations, however, are likely to be relative, with many experiments, for example, linking all three DMN sub-networks to conscious recollection (Vilberg and Rugg, 2012).
Here, we consider a simple conceptualisation of DMN function and apply it within the Andrews-Hanna framework. Arguably, imagination, mind-wandering, and taking another's perspective have in common a substantial change from the current cognitive context. Similarly, conscious recollection is typically conceived as reactivation of a previously-experienced episode, with components linked into a complex surrounding, context. Substantial shifts of context may be common in everyday activity, for example, a shift from cooking dinner to giving directions to guests over the phone, but less common in the constrained setting of typical laboratory tasks. For example, in a recent review of neuroimaging studies of task switching (Kim et al., 2011), tasks that they argue required a contextual switch involved either a change in simple attended features or binary categorization rules using a fixed, small set of possible stimuli. Irrespective of specific high-level processes involved, we reasoned that the DMN may be involved in any large switch of cognitive context—an operation presumably calling for relaxation of many aspects of a current attentional focus, with concomitant activation of representations and processes relevant to the new context.
To test this hypothesis, we used a novel experimental paradigm that required participants to switch between similar and dissimilar tasks, within a relatively large set of six tasks (Figure 1). The six tasks were each associated with a different rule, as determined by the colour border surrounding the task stimulus. The tasks were split into three groups defined by stimulus category, with two possible tasks per stimulus type. A no-switch trial occurred when participants had to apply the same rule that was applied on the previous trial. A similar-task-switch trial—resembling switches in typical neuroimaging studies—occurred when participants had to apply the other rule from the same category as the previous trial. A dissimilar-task-switch occurred when participants had to apply a rule from a different category compared to the previous trial.
Contrary to the common concept of a task-negative system, we predicted increased DMN recruitment for the most difficult condition of switching between dissimilar tasks. We found this to be the case and furthermore, that the activity increase was selectively found in the Core and MTL sub-networks. In addition, we provide evidence that all sub-networks of the DMN represented task-related information during task performance.
Results
Behavioral results
Accuracy on all tasks was high (median accuracy for all tasks >95%, inter quartile range <6%). As predicted, response time (RT) was significantly longer when switching between dissimilar tasks (2043 ms) compared both to trials when no task switch occurred (1670 ms; t17 = 8.6, p < 0.001), and to trials with switches between similar tasks (1746 ms; t17 = 8.1, p < 0.001). Switches between similar tasks also produced significantly longer RTs compared to no-switch trials (t17 = 2.8, p = 0.006).
Task-switch related activity in the DMN
Preprocessing steps for fMRI data included realignment of the raw echo-planar images (EPI), slice-time correction, coregistration of the EPI images with the structural image, normalisation to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template brain, smoothing with an 8 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel and filtering with a high-pass filter (see ‘Materials and methods’). Univariate analysis of fMRI data was used to compare dissimilar-task-switch with no-switch trials through the standard general linear model (GLM) approach. A regressor was constructed for each switch type with events modelled from stimulus onset until response and convolved with the haemodynamic response function. The resulting beta values for each switch type were compared and thresholded at p < 0.05, correcting for the false discovery rate. We identified widespread activation predominantly in regions of in the DMN (Figure 2A), with peaks found in bilateral HF, PHC, Rsp, PCC, AMPFC, and left pIPL (Table 1). It is worth noting that all of these regions fall within either the Core or MTL sub-networks. In comparison, no regions from the DMPFC sub-network showed significant activation at the whole-brain level. A contrast of similar-task-switch against no-switch trials revealed no significant activation across the whole brain.
To examine changes in activation from the perspective of the DMN sub-networks, we used individual DMN regions of interest (ROIs) previously defined (Buckner et al., 2009; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010). The mean beta value was extracted from each ROI following each switch type. Planned, paired two-tailed t-tests revealed significant increase in activity during dissimilar-task-switch compared to no-switch in core (bilateral AMPFC, PCC) and MTL (Rsp, PHC) sub-networks, with a tendency to de-activation in the DMPFC sub-network (significant in right TPJ) (Figure 2C). Again, no ROIs revealed a significant difference between the similar-task-switch trials and no-switch trials. Two-way repeated measures ANOVAs were performed separately for each sub-network, with factors of ROI (Core: 4, MTL: 9, DMPFC: 7) and switch type (no-switch, dissimilar-task-switch). Main effects of task-switching were found for the Core (F(1,17) = 16.7, p = 0.001) and MTL (F(1,17) = 6.1, p = 0.03) sub-networks, showing increased activity for dissimilar switches. In contrast, the DMPFC sub-network showed a marginally significant de-activation (F(1,17) = 4.1, p = 0.06). Corresponding ANOVAs were performed to test for the difference between similar-task-switch and no-switch, but these revealed no main effect in any sub-network. To investigate differences at the sub-network level, beta values were averaged across the ROIs within each sub-network each of the three trial types, and a two-way repeated measures ANOVA (factors of sub-network and switch type) was performed on the mean beta values. This analysis revealed a main effect of sub-network (F(2,34) = 18.9, p < 0.001) and an interaction of switch type and sub-network (F(4,68) = 17.8, p < 0.001). These data therefore show a dissociation within the DMN: While the DMPFC sub-network displayed the characteristic pattern of reduced activity during executive control, switching between dissimilar tasks showed an opposite pattern of increased activity in Core and MTL sub-networks.
In an exploratory analysis, we looked at the univariate activation associated with dissimilar switches between specific categories compared to no-switch trials, in the three DMN sub-networks, for example, from a semantic task to a lexical task, compared with repetition of the lexical task. This was performed for all six between category switch types. Figure 3 shows that all types of switch showed a relative increase in Core sub-network activation and decrease in DMFPC sub-network activation. The MTL sub-network shows increases for 4 of the 6 switch types and a marginal decrease when switching from the perceptual task. Especially for the Core and DMPFC sub-networks, these data suggest little variation in the pattern of results across different task types.
Multivariate decoding demonstrates task representation across the DMN
For the multivariate analysis, the same preprocessing pipeline was followed with the omission of the smoothing step. We reasoned that if the DMN was involved in switching between tasks, then the differences between tasks might be represented within the network. To test this hypothesis, we performed a multivariate pattern analysis on the same ROIs. For each ROI, classifiers were trained to discriminate between the voxel-wise pattern of activation for each task pair (6 tasks, therefore 15 task pairs) and these classifiers were subsequently tested on independent data using leave-one-run-out cross-validation (see ‘Materials and methods’). The matrices in Figure 4A show the classification accuracy (CA) for each task pair in each ROI, averaged across participants. The strongest decoding of task was found in bilateral HF, pIPL and the PCC, while bilateral TPJ, Rsp, AMPFC, and DMPFC on the midline showed weaker but still significant decoding.
Decoding of task between dissimilar task pairs is likely driven by many differences in task features. In contrast, differences between similar tasks will be predominantly driven by the internal representation of the specific decision rule. To quantify the extent to which rule and other features were driving the CA scores, CAs for ROIs within the Core, MTL and DMPFC sub-networks were averaged separately for similar task pairs and dissimilar task pairs. This analysis (Figure 4B) revealed significant decoding of dissimilar task pairs in all DMN sub-networks, and weaker but significant decoding of similar task pairs in the DMPFC sub-network.
Recently concern has arisen that differences in RT may be driving differences in CA (Todd et al., 2013; for contrary arguments see; Woolgar et al., 2014). We performed a regression analysis of CA against absolute difference in RT in each of the three sub-networks separately. First, we extracted the CA associated with each task pair in each ROI in each subject. We then calculated the mean CA across the component ROIs of the Core, MTL, and DMPFC sub-networks in each individual, producing a 3-dimensional matrix of CA values for 3 sub-networks × 18 subjects × 15 task pairs. A similar matrix was produced for absolute RT differences. We segregated the dissimilar–task pairs and similar–task pairs and conducted a Spearman's correlational analysis of RT against CA for each task-pair type within each sub-network. The results showed strong discrimination of dissimilar task pairs, and weak discrimination of similar task pairs, irrespective of RT difference (Figure 5A).
A second analysis considered data from each subject separately, with a separate regression analysis for similar and dissimilar task pairs in each ROI. A general linear model was constructed with a regressor for the absolute RT difference for either the similar-task-pairs or dissimilar-task-pairs, which was fit to the corresponding CA data. This produced a beta estimate for similar and dissimilar-task-pairs in each ROI, in each subject. Beta estimates were subsequently averaged across the component ROIs of each sub-network. Figure 5B shows the mean beta estimate for the similar and dissimilar task pairs in each subject for the Core, MTL, and DMPFC sub-networks. Within each graph, data from the 18 subjects are sorted in ascending order. Overall, the graphs suggest that RT does not systematically predict CA across participants, especially for the dissimilar task pairs (bottom row) for which CA was highest.
Together these analyses demonstrate that simple RT differences were not strongly driving the classification accuracies.
Discussion
In previous work, DMN activity has been associated with a variety of complex, often self-referential mental processes, such as retrieving past events from one's life, imagining possible future events, or considering the beliefs of oneself and others (Buckner and Carroll, 2007). Here, we argue that a simple variable may relate these complex processes—the degree of change in cognitive context. To address this hypothesis we modified a typical task switching study to include both small shifts—similar to those of many previous studies—and much larger shifts. In contrast to the common idea of the DMN as a task-negative system, the activity of which progressively decreases with increasing task difficulty, our results show the opposite for a large change of cognitive context, in particular for Core and MTL sub-networks.
As cognitive context is changed, elements of the old context must be suppressed and elements of the new context must be retrieved or activated, producing a reconfiguration appropriate to the new circumstances. Our data suggest DMN activation only when the change is sufficiently large, perhaps analogous to many of the shifts taking place in everyday cognition. It is uncertain whether large and small shifts differ qualitatively or only quantitatively. In our task, for example, a shift between rules within the same category may have been executed without reference to the link between frame color and categorization rule; to perform such a switch, it was necessary only to see that frame color had changed and to retrieve the other rule relevant to the current set of stimuli. In contrast, a shift between categories likely required reference back to a broader set of task rules, including the remembered list of color–rule combinations. More broadly, one possibility is that the DMN is involved in relaxing a current attentional focus, allowing new cognitive contents to arise. When cognitive operations are largely similar across successive trials, the DMN may be suppressed, but as increasingly more of the current focus must be dissolved, suppression may shift to activation.
Thus, it is the magnitude of switch involved that distinguishes this study from much of the previous work in the literature. The point is illustrated by a detailed consideration of the task-switching review by Kim et al. (2011), as described in the introduction. Kim et al. (2011) identified three major types of task-switch found in the literature: perceptual switch, response switch, and contextual switch, the latter of which is the most comparable to the current work. Of the 20 tasks that they identified as involving a contextual switch, 17 used fixed small stimulus sets, with the switches (like our within-category switches) concerning just the relevant stimulus feature such as colour or shape. Two studies used a task that required participants to make one of two possible binary decisions on serially presented letter strings. The remaining task was essentially our semantic task, using text instead of images. Unlike these studies, we propose that it is our use of much more substantial switches, requiring larger revisions of cognitive context and operations, that leads to the novel finding of DMN activation.
Much previous work links activity in the DMN to conscious recollection, commonly defined by the ability to link a remembered item to the surrounding context of a specific event. A role in memory for individual events fits well with the proposal that the DMN binds together the components of a complex cognitive context. Beyond traditional studies of episodic and autobiographical memory, our results show the importance of DMN context processing in cognitive control. In contrast to the common finding of deactivation linked to executive control, our findings show increased activity when the change of context is sufficiently large. Establishing a new, complex context, we suggest, may be common to recollection of specific previous events and to major revisions of complex task rules.
An intriguing aspect of the present results is the difference between the regions that show switch-related activity and those that encoded task-related information. The regions that demonstrate switch-related activity show relatively strong correspondence to the pattern of DMN fractionation presented by Andrews-Hanna et al. In contrast, the DMN regions that show strong encoding extend across all three sub-networks, suggesting only partial functional segregation. Encoding analysis is likely more sensitive than univariate activity analysis, reflecting both overall activity within a broad region, and the exact pattern of activity within that region (Davis et al., 2014). Though DMPFC showed no univariate signal linked to switching task category, our results suggest some involvement in task representation.
Our results show very different levels of CA for discrimination of similar vs dissimilar tasks. For the comparison of similar tasks, other than the different colour of border surrounding the task item, the only difference is the internal representation of task rule. In contrast, comparing dissimilar tasks involves a myriad of differences such as visual properties of the stimuli, cognitive domain (semantic knowledge, lexicon, visual discrimination) as well as task rule. Correspondingly, much stronger decoding is seen across the DMN between dissimilar tasks. This is unsurprising for a network of regions hypothesised to link components of a broad cognitive context.
Many studies suggest sustained DMN activity in rest compared to active task performance. It is unclear how such sustained activity relates to the switch-related activity we have shown here. On the one hand, it is plausible that, when a participant lies in a scanner at rest, there are periodic large shifts in the content of cognition, and in part, ‘sustained’ DMN activity may reflect averaging across shifts occurring at variable, unknown times. Indeed, traditional studies of resting state functional connectivity depend on temporal variation in network activity, as expected for a signal in part linked to transient cognitive events. That said, if a core aspect of DMN function is relaxing an attentional focus, sustained enhancement is plausible during a period of relatively unfocused cognitive activity.
An unexpected aspect of our results is the lack of switch-associated activity when changing between similar tasks, which does show a robust behavioral cost compared to no-switch trials. In previous studies, apparently similar cases of task switching have been associated with widespread recruitment of a fronto-parietal, executive control network (Sohn et al., 2000; Braver et al., 2003; Monsell, 2003; Yeung et al., 2006; Kim et al., 2011), and it is unclear why no similar activity was seen in our data. One contributing factor may be our explicit modelling of RT differences between conditions, convolving the canonical haemodynamic response with the duration of each trial from stimulus presentation until response. As this procedure is designed to correct for activation differences due simply to longer RT on switch trials, it may reduce or remove differences seen in studies that do not adopt such a correction. Our results also raise the possibility, however, that traditional task-switching results may not generalise to the more complex conditions of our experiment. Future work will be needed to resolve this discrepancy.
Conclusion
In conclusion, we propose that the DMN may be recruited whenever large changes of cognitive context are required. This may apply in complex cases of self-referential processing, mind wandering etc, but also in relatively simple acts of cognitive or executive control. The DMN, widely seen as a ‘task-negative’ network, may respond positively to any task which demands a switch from one broad context to another.
Materials and methods
Participants
18 right-handed participants (10 females) aged between 18 and 40 were recruited from the Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit subject panel. Of 21 original subjects scanned, three had to be removed for excessive head-movements (over 10 mm translation and/or 6° rotation). No participant had a history of neurological or psychiatric illness. Participants were reimbursed for their time. Ethics approval was given by the Cambridge Psychology Research Ethics Committee.
Task description
Request a detailed protocolAll three tasks were created using the Psychophysics Toolbox for MATLAB (Brainard, 1997). Within the scanner, the stimulus display was projected onto a mirror mounted to a 32-channel head-coil.
Participants were required to learn six different tasks (Figure 1A). Each task was associated with a different rule, with the appropriate rule determined by the colour border in which the task stimulus appeared. The six tasks rules are shown in Figure 1A. Prior to scanning, participants practised the task until they had completed at least 20 trials with an accuracy exceeding 80%. Importantly, the six tasks were grouped into three categories of two tasks each, where the stimuli within a category could be relevant to either rule within that category, but not to rules of other categories. Furthermore, all categories included trials which required a positive answer for both rules, for one rule but not the other, or for neither rule; therefore subjects needed to remember and apply the correct rule on all trials. All questions were framed in a true/false format, so that arbitrary response mappings for each rule did not have to be learned in addition to the rules themselves.
Each trial began with the simultaneous appearance of the colour border (visual angle = 7.9°) and the task stimulus (Figure 1B). Participants were requested to respond as quickly as possible with a true or false answer (right thumb button press = true, left thumb button press = false). The border and stimulus remained on screen until the subject had made their response. A low tone was played to participants if they made an incorrect response. There was a jittered interval between the response to one trial and the onset of the next. Interval jittering followed an exponential distribution between 1 s and 11 s, with a mean of 4.1 s.
Participants learned the tasks prior to scanning. An event-related design was adopted, with 73 trials per run. Each run had at least 12 trials of each of the six task types. Task switch type was also balanced within a run: 24 no-switch trials, 24 similar-task-switches, and 24 dissimilar-task-switches. Post scanning, when questioned, no participants reported having any sense of what task to expect on a given trial.
fMRI acquisition
Request a detailed protocolScans were acquired with a 3T Siemens Trim Trio scanner. 32 3-mm slices (0.75 mm interslice gap) in axial orientation gave an in-plane resolution of 3 × 3 mm and were acquired using a TR of 2 s. T2*-weighted EPI capturing blood oxygen level dependent contrast was employed with a flip angle of 78°. For both experiments, the first eight images were discarded to avoid T1 equilibration effects.
Univariate analysis
Request a detailed protocolFor the univariate analysis, images were preprocessed and analysed with SPM5 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology). In the first preprocessing step, data were checked for obvious artefacts, and all images were realigned to the first image. Next we performed slice time correction and coregistration of the structural with the functional EPI images. Finally, data were normalized to the standard MNI template, smoothed with an 8 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel and subjected to a high-pass filter with cut-off at 128 s.
Fixed-effects analyses were performed on each individual's data using a general linear model. In the first univariate analysis investigating switching related activity three regressor functions were used (no-switch trials, similar-task-switch trials, and dissimilar-task-switch trials). Each regressor was modelled as a rectangular function from the onset of each stimulus to the moment of response and convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function. Beta weight images were contrasted for the conditions dissimilar-task-switch > no-switch and similar-task-switch > no-switch. Contrasts were further examined by random-effects analysis. Activation maps (threshold 0.05, FDR-corrected) were visualised using MRIcroGL (Rorden et al., 2007).
For ROI analysis, mean contrast values within each ROI were extracted for each subject, using the MarsBaR SPM toolbox (Brett et al., 2002). ROIs were spherical, with 8 mm radius, based around peak coordinates (Figure 2B) taken from Andrews-Hanna et al. (2010).
For the exploratory analysis into the activation associated with switches between specific categories a separate GLM was constructed. 36 regressors were used (one regressor for each possible switch type) and modelled as before. The resulting beta estimates were then processed using the same ROI analysis method as before with the same ROIs.
Multivoxel pattern analysis
Request a detailed protocolMultivoxel pattern analysis was performed using the Decoding Toolbox (Christophel et al., 2012; Görgen et al., 2012). Preprocessing of the data was the same as for the univariate whole-brain analysis, except for the omission of the smoothing step. Again, a fixed effects analysis was performed on each participant's data using a general linear model. For this GLM, each task was modelled as a separate regressor, constructed as a rectangular function from the onset of each stimulus to the moment of response and convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function. The same ROIs as previously (Figure 2B) were used.
Prior to pattern analysis, beta values were Z-scored across tasks within each voxel of the ROI. This step was intended to reduce any impact of task differences in overall ROI activity. Pattern discrimination between tasks was then estimated using pairwise classification, that is, only 1 of the 15 possible task pairs was decoded at a time. A support vector machine (LIBSVM) (Fan et al., 2005) was used to train and classify data from three of the four runs, with the remaining run used to test the classifier. Test and training runs were always kept separate and each run was used to test the classifier once, that is, fourfold cross-validation. The CA for a given ROI was averaged across test-train splits, yielding a single CA for each ROI, in each individual, for each task pair.
References
-
Self-projection and the brainTrends in Cognitive Sciences 11:49–57.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004
-
Decoding the contents of visual short-term memory from human visual and parietal cortexThe Journal of Neuroscience 32:12983–12989.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0184-12.2012
-
Working set selection using second order information for training support vector machinesThe Journal of Machine Learning Research 6:1889–1918.
-
Does ‘task difficulty’ explain ‘Task-Induced deactivation’?Frontiers in Psychology 3:125.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00125
-
18th International conference on functional mapping of the human brain (HBM)18th International conference on functional mapping of the human brain (HBM), 5812.
-
Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain functionProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA 98:4259–4264.https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.071043098
-
Deconstructing episodic memory with constructionTrends in Cognitive Sciences 11:299–306.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.001
-
Task switchingTrends in Cognitive Sciences 7:134–140.https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7
-
A default mode of brain functionProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98:676–682.https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.98.2.676
-
A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea1083–1090, Neuroimage, 37, discussion 1097–9, 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.041.
-
Improving lesion-symptom mappingJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19:1081–1088.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1081
-
Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. decreases in cerebral cortexJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9:648–663.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648
-
The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switchingProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 97:13448–13453.https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.240460497
-
The neural correlates of recollection: transient versus sustained FMRI effectsThe Journal of Neuroscience 32:15679–15687.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3065-12.2012
-
Coherent spontaneous activity identifies a hippocampal-parietal memory networkJournal of Neurophysiology 96:3517–3531.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00048.2006
-
Between-task competition and cognitive control in task switchingThe Journal of Neuroscience 26:1429–1438.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3109-05.2006
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Medical Research Council (MRC) (MC_US_A060_0001)
- John Duncan
The funder had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
This work was funded by the Medical Research Council (UK) intramural program MC_US_A060_0001.
Ethics
Human subjects: Informed consent, and consent to publish, was obtained through the University of Cambridge ethics committee: CPREC (Cambridge Psychology Research Ethics) 2010.16.
Copyright
© 2015, Crittenden et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,059
- views
-
- 653
- downloads
-
- 136
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
The remarkable ability of the motor system to adapt to novel environments has traditionally been investigated using kinematically non-redundant tasks, such as planar reaching movements. This limitation prevents the study of how the motor system achieves adaptation by altering the movement patterns of our redundant body. To address this issue, we developed a redundant motor task in which participants reached for targets with the tip of a virtual stick held with both hands. Despite the redundancy of the task, participants consistently employed a stereotypical strategy of flexibly changing the tilt angle of the stick depending on the direction of tip movement. Thus, this baseline relationship between tip-movement direction and stick-tilt angle constrained both the physical and visual movement patterns of the redundant system. Our task allowed us to systematically investigate how the motor system implicitly changed both the tip-movement direction and the stick-tilt angle in response to imposed visual perturbations. Both types of perturbations, whether directly affecting the task (tip-movement direction) or not (stick-tilt angle around the tip), drove adaptation, and the patterns of implicit adaptation were guided by the baseline relationship. Consequently, tip-movement adaptation was associated with changes in stick-tilt angle, and intriguingly, even seemingly ignorable stick-tilt perturbations significantly influenced tip-movement adaptation, leading to tip-movement direction errors. These findings provide a new understanding that the baseline relationship plays a crucial role not only in how the motor system controls movement of the redundant system, but also in how it implicitly adapts to modify movement patterns.