Abstract

Seizures are often followed by sensory, cognitive or motor impairments during the postictal phase that show striking similarity to transient hypoxic/ischemic attacks. Here we show that seizures result in a severe hypoxic attack confined to the postictal period. We measured brain oxygenation in localized areas from freely-moving rodents and discovered a severe hypoxic event (pO2<10mmHg) after the termination of seizures. This event lasted over an hour, is mediated by hypoperfusion, generalizes to people with epilepsy, and is attenuated by inhibiting cyclooxygenase-2 or L-type calcium channels. Using inhibitors of these targets we separated the seizure from the resulting severe hypoxia and show that structure specific postictal memory and behavioral impairments are the consequence of this severe hypoperfusion/hypoxic event. Thus, epilepsy is much more than a disease hallmarked by seizures, since the occurrence of postictal hypoperfusion/hypoxia results in a separate set of neurological consequences that are currently not being treated and are preventable.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Jordan S Farrell

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  2. Ismael Gaxiola-Valdez

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Marshal D Wolff

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Laurence S David

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Haruna I Dika

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. Bryce L Geeraert

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  7. X Rachel Wang

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  8. Shaily Singh

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  9. Simon C Spanswick

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  10. Jeff F Dunn

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  11. Michael C Antle

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-5178-4683
  12. Paolo Federico

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    For correspondence
    pfederic@ucalgary.ca
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  13. Gordon Campbell Teskey

    Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada
    For correspondence
    gteskey@ucalgary.ca
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-8462-355X

Funding

Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOP-130495)

  • Gordon Campbell Teskey

Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (RGPIN/03819-2014)

  • Gordon Campbell Teskey

Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOP-136839)

  • Paolo Federico

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Rodents were handled and maintained according to the Canadian Council for Animal Care guidelines. These procedures were approved by the Life and Environmental Sciences Animal Care and Health Sciences Animal Care Committees at the University of Calgary (AC11-0073).

Human subjects: Human experimentation was approved by the University of Calgary's Conjoint Health Research Ethics Board (REB13-0571). All patients (or guardians of patients) provided written informed consent.

Copyright

© 2016, Farrell et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 6,534
    views
  • 558
    downloads
  • 111
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Jordan S Farrell
  2. Ismael Gaxiola-Valdez
  3. Marshal D Wolff
  4. Laurence S David
  5. Haruna I Dika
  6. Bryce L Geeraert
  7. X Rachel Wang
  8. Shaily Singh
  9. Simon C Spanswick
  10. Jeff F Dunn
  11. Michael C Antle
  12. Paolo Federico
  13. Gordon Campbell Teskey
(2016)
Postictal behavioural impairments are due to a severe prolonged hypoperfusion/hypoxia event that is COX-2 dependent
eLife 5:e19352.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.19352

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.19352

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    2. Physics of Living Systems
    Moritz Schloetter, Georg U Maret, Christoph J Kleineidam
    Research Article

    Neurons generate and propagate electrical pulses called action potentials which annihilate on arrival at the axon terminal. We measure the extracellular electric field generated by propagating and annihilating action potentials and find that on annihilation, action potentials expel a local discharge. The discharge at the axon terminal generates an inhomogeneous electric field that immediately influences target neurons and thus provokes ephaptic coupling. Our measurements are quantitatively verified by a powerful analytical model which reveals excitation and inhibition in target neurons, depending on position and morphology of the source-target arrangement. Our model is in full agreement with experimental findings on ephaptic coupling at the well-studied Basket cell-Purkinje cell synapse. It is able to predict ephaptic coupling for any other synaptic geometry as illustrated by a few examples.

    1. Neuroscience
    Sven Ohl, Martin Rolfs
    Research Article

    Detecting causal relations structures our perception of events in the world. Here, we determined for visual interactions whether generalized (i.e. feature-invariant) or specialized (i.e. feature-selective) visual routines underlie the perception of causality. To this end, we applied a visual adaptation protocol to assess the adaptability of specific features in classical launching events of simple geometric shapes. We asked observers to report whether they observed a launch or a pass in ambiguous test events (i.e. the overlap between two discs varied from trial to trial). After prolonged exposure to causal launch events (the adaptor) defined by a particular set of features (i.e. a particular motion direction, motion speed, or feature conjunction), observers were less likely to see causal launches in subsequent ambiguous test events than before adaptation. Crucially, adaptation was contingent on the causal impression in launches as demonstrated by a lack of adaptation in non-causal control events. We assessed whether this negative aftereffect transfers to test events with a new set of feature values that were not presented during adaptation. Processing in specialized (as opposed to generalized) visual routines predicts that the transfer of visual adaptation depends on the feature similarity of the adaptor and the test event. We show that the negative aftereffects do not transfer to unadapted launch directions but do transfer to launch events of different speeds. Finally, we used colored discs to assign distinct feature-based identities to the launching and the launched stimulus. We found that the adaptation transferred across colors if the test event had the same motion direction as the adaptor. In summary, visual adaptation allowed us to carve out a visual feature space underlying the perception of causality and revealed specialized visual routines that are tuned to a launch’s motion direction.