Laminar microcircuitry of visual cortex producing attention-associated electric fields
Abstract
Cognitive operations are widely studied by measuring electric fields through EEG and ECoG. However, despite their widespread use, the neural circuitry giving rise to these signals remains unknown because the functional architecture of cortical columns producing attention-associated electric fields has not been explored. Here, we detail the laminar cortical circuitry underlying an attention-associated electric field measured over posterior regions of the brain in humans and monkeys. First, we identified visual cortical area V4 as one plausible contributor to this attention-associated electric field through inverse modeling of cranial EEG in macaque monkeys performing a visual attention task. Next, we performed laminar neurophysiological recordings on the prelunate gyrus and identified the electric-field-producing dipoles as synaptic activity in distinct cortical layers of area V4. Specifically, activation in the extragranular layers of cortex resulted in the generation of the attention-associated dipole. Feature selectivity of a given cortical column determined the overall contribution to this electric field. Columns selective for the attended feature contributed more to the electric field than columns selective for a different feature. Last, the laminar profile of synaptic activity generated by V4 was sufficient to produce an attention-associated signal measurable outside of the column. These findings suggest that the top-down recipient cortical layers produce an attention-associated electric field that can be measured extracortically with the relative contribution of each column depending upon the underlying functional architecture.
Editor's evaluation
By combining rare EEG and laminar recordings in monkeys, Westerberg and colleagues studied the neural correlates of the well-known attention-related N2pc signal and found that it is due to the activation of extra-granular layers of cortex. Further, this effect was stronger for columns that were more feature selective. These findings are extremely important and a unique contribution to the literature on the neurobiology of attention.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.72139.sa0Introduction
Research into extracortical electric fields provides fundamental insights into the mechanisms of human perception, cognition, and intention. For instance, event-related potential (ERP) components like the N2pc (Eimer, 1996; Luck and Hillyard, 1994; Woodman and Luck, 1999) and Pd (Hickey et al., 2009) reliably index selective attention in humans and monkeys, alike. However, the interpretation of these extracortical measures of attention is severely limited by uncertainty about the exact neural processes that generate these signals (Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006). Understanding what brain processes an electric field indicates requires knowing how it is generated (e.g., Cohen, 2017).
One avenue to localize neural generators of electric fields is through inverse source localization (Michel et al., 2004; Grech et al., 2008). However, the results are indefinite and cannot offer conclusive answers. Moreover, these methods do not allow for the probing of the underlying neural circuitry. For example, most EEG signals are hypothesized to be generated by interlaminar interactions in cortical columns (Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006). Columnar microcircuits are ubiquitous across the brain (Douglas et al., 1989; Douglas and Martin, 1991), having a well-defined anatomical structure (Mountcastle, 1997; Kaas, 2012) and consistent physiological activation pattern (Bastos et al., 2012 but see Godlove et al., 2014). The canonical cortical microcircuit offers a framework in which to interpret columnar dynamics in sensory or cognitive tasks, yet the relationship between this functional architecture and electric fields related to cognition commonly measured in humans is unexplored.
Electric fields measured at the surface of the brain (ECoG) and scalp (EEG) are theorized to be generated by dipoles in cortex. However, measuring current dipoles requires sampling electrical potentials across all the layers of the cerebral cortex. Such laminar neurophysiological measurements are rare and unsystematic in humans. Work in rodents has uncovered intriguing insights into cortical laminar microcircuits underlying evoked EEG signals, but all of these were limited to sensory responses (Jellema et al., 2004; Bruyns-Haylett et al., 2017; Nass et al., 2021). Fortunately, macaque monkeys produce homologues of the attention-associated EEG signals (N2pc: Woodman et al., 2007; Cohen et al., 2009; Purcell et al., 2013; Pd: Cosman et al., 2018). Laminar neurophysiological measurements (Schroeder et al., 1998; Maier et al., 2010; Buffalo et al., 2011; Hansen et al., 2011; Self et al., 2013; Godlove et al., 2014; Engel et al., 2016; Klein et al., 2016; Hembrook-Short et al., 2017; Nandy et al., 2017; Trautmann et al., 2019; Westerberg et al., 2019; Tovar et al., 2020; Ferro et al., 2021) and EEG (Schmid et al., 2006; Woodman et al., 2007; Sandhaeger et al., 2019) are well established in macaques. However, despite many studies linking intra- and extracortical signals (Schroeder et al., 1992; Whittingstall and Logothetis, 2009; Musall et al., 2014; Snyder and Smith, 2015), to date, little is known about the laminar origins of ERPs in primates.
Here, we show that visual cortex generates dipoles through layer-specific transsynaptic currents that give rise to electric fields that track the deployment of selective attention. These dipoles were generated by the extragranular compartments of cortex, indicating these cognitive operations likely arise from top-down interactions. Moreover, functional architecture – in the form of feature columns – was associated with the relative contribution of individual, local cortical columns to the global electric field. These results are the first to our knowledge to describe laminar specificity in synaptic activations contributing to the generation of electric fields associated with cognitive processing.
Results
Attention task
To investigate extracortical manifestations of attention-associated electric fields, we trained macaque monkeys to perform a visual search task (Figure 1A). Three macaque monkeys (designated Ca, He, and Z) performed visual search for an oddball color target (red or green), presented within an array of five or seven uniform distractors (green or red) (N sessions for each monkey: Ca 21, He 9, Z 18). A fourth monkey (P) performed visual search for an oddball shape (T or L) presented within an array of up to seven uniform distractors (L or T) (N sessions: monkey P, 22). Each animal performed well above chance (chance level for monkeys Ca, He: 16.6%; P, Z: 12.5%) (behavioral accuracy in color search: Ca 88%, He 81%, Z 85%, shape search monkey P 66%). We sampled cortical neural signals during the color pop-out search to be certain of which item received the benefit of attention in the array. We used the more difficult search data to determine the generality of our findings. Two different recording types were used, necessitating four monkeys total, as described below and summarized in Supplementary file 1.
Inverse modeling of attention-associated extracortical electric fields points to visual cortex
Once animals could perform visual search, we implanted an array of electrodes approximating the human 10–20 system in monkeys P and Z (Figure 1A). Using these electrodes, we observed extracortical electric dynamics in both monkeys. An index of attention known as the N2pc manifests during visual search. The N2pc electric field indexes attention allocation in this task. The magnitude of the N2pc was largest over occipital sites (Figure 1B, Figure 1—figure supplement 1), consistent with previous reports in humans and macaques (Luck and Hillyard, 1994; Eimer, 1996; Woodman and Luck, 1999; Hopf et al., 2000; Woodman et al., 2007; Cohen et al., 2009; Purcell et al., 2013). We used sLORETA inverse modeling for source localization. Previous source estimates for the N2pc identified the human homologue of V4 (Luck and Hillyard, 1990; Luck and Hillyard, 1994; Hopf et al., 2000). These findings are consistent with numerous reports that areas in mid-level visual cortex in monkeys produce robust attention signals (Moran and Desimone, 1985; Luck et al., 1997a; McAdams and Maunsell, 1999; Reynolds et al., 1999; Fries et al., 2001; see Roe et al., 2012 for review) across cortical layers (Engel et al., 2016; Nandy et al., 2017). Consistent with these earlier studies, the inverse model showed that current sources include V4 on the prelunate gyrus (Figure 1C, Figure 1—figure supplement 1). However, the modeled current sources also included other cortical regions, as is common for inverse solutions. Notably, the inverse solution identifies V1 to be about as strong as V4 in contributing to the N2pc, which is unlikely given current knowledge on attentional modulation for each area (Motter, 1993; Luck et al., 1997a; Kastner et al., 1999; Buffalo et al., 2011). Given the primary feature used in the search task was color, we investigated the laminar profile of attention-associated electric field generation in V4 where color is better represented (Roe et al., 2012).
V4’s laminar microcircuit produces dipoles that predict the attention-associated electric field
Guided by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), linear multielectrode arrays (LMAs) were inserted into area V4 of monkeys Ca and He. LMAs were placed perpendicular to the cortical surface, spanning supragranular (L2/3), granular (L4), and infragranular (L5/6) cortical layers (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). We confirmed that attentional modulation of spiking activity could be observed during pop-out search performance consistent with previous reports (Westerberg et al., 2020a). Moreover, the laminar profile of attentional modulation matched that of attentional modulation in a different task with spiking activity in the middle layers being the most highly enhanced with attention (Figure 2; Nandy et al., 2017). Critically, while attentional modulation is present in the laminar data prior to the emergence of extracortical attention-associated fields such as the N2pc, that cross-laminar modulation persists through this interval.
Simultaneous with LMA recording, an extracortical electric signal was recorded immediately above V4 – critically the recording took place outside of the cortical column itself. Current source density (CSD) was derived from the local field potentials (LFPs) sampled across V4 layers. To relate the extracortical signal (Figure 3A) to synaptic currents estimated as CSD (Figure 3B–D), we employed information theory to capture multivariate factors and nonlinearities between signals (Shannon, 1948; Cover and Thomas, 2006). Importantly, information theory analyses are model independent (Timme and Lapish, 2018). Information theory is thus superior to standard linear models since these models cannot capture all potential relationships between signals. The relationship between the extracortical signal and CSD was assessed in four distinct steps, as illustrated by a representative session (Figure 3E–F, Figure 3—figure supplement 1). We use the interval of the N2pc to determine whether laminar circuitry in V4 can contribute to the attention-associated electric fields. This interval occurred before the median response times for each monkey contributing laminar V4 data ([median± standard deviation]: monkey Ca 227 ± 49 ms, He 225 ± 44 ms).
First, we employed Monte Carlo simulations of the mutual information analysis to verify that the extracortical signal exhibited significantly enhanced information about target position during the time window of the N2pc. Second, we measured target information across the layers of V4 during the N2pc interval. This analysis revealed enhanced information in L2/3 and L5/6 but not in L4. Third, we computed the mutual information between the extracortical signal and CSD during the N2pc window, irrespective of target position. This analysis showed a significant relationship between the extracortical signal and the CSD in L2/3 and L5/6 but not in L4. Fourth, we measured the transmitted information about target location from CSD to extracortical signal during the N2pc interval (Timme and Lapish, 2018). This analysis demonstrated significant information transmission to the extracortical signal from L2/3 and L5/6, but not L4.
Averaged across sessions, we observed that the electric field during the N2pc interval (Figure 4A) was associated with a consistent CSD pattern (Figure 4B). This relationship was observed in each monkey (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Presentation of the search array in any configuration elicited an early current sink in L4, followed by a prolonged sink in L2/3 that was associated with a briefer source in L5/6.
We next computed information transmission about target location from the CSD to the extracortical signal for each session (Figure 4C). All cortical layers provided significant information transmission in >75% of sessions during the N2pc window (150–190 ms following array onset). However, the magnitude of transmitted target information was significantly greater in L2/3 and L5/6 relative to L4 (L2/3-L4: t(29) = 2.15, p = 0.040; L5/6-L4: t(29) = 2.20, p = 0.036). The magnitude of information transmission was not significantly different between L2/3 and L5/6 (t(29) = 0.21, p = 0.84). Across sessions, the three other information theoretic analyses were consistent with the example session (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Moreover, significant information transmission during the N2pc was observed in each monkey (Figure 4—figure supplement 1).
To verify the results, we applied the information theoretic analysis over a longer interval (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). Importantly, we found no signal differences or significant information transmission in the 100 ms pre-array baseline period as expected with baseline correction. We also evaluated the interval 200–250 ms following array presentation and found a polarization reversal in the extracortical signal likely corresponding to the Pd (Cosman et al., 2018). We observe persistent current differences in the extragranular CSD during this interval sufficient to contribute to the extracortical signal. However, we observed no statistically significant information theoretic relationship between the CSD and extracortical signal during this interval. The absence of a relationship could indicate no actual association or be a consequence of the reduced trial count due to the clipping of signals at saccade initiation. This uncertainty prevents further consideration of this interval in these data.
Last, we performed two additional analyses to determine whether the observed information theoretic relationship is confounded by spurious factors. First, we measured the contribution of V4 neuron selectivity for stimulus color. We computed information transmission separately for trials with a red stimulus and with a green stimulus in the receptive field (RF). In the population average of the two calculations for each session, we observed significant information transmission during the N2pc (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). Hence, the relationship between V4 activity and the EEG does not depend on color specificity. Second, we measured the contribution of microsaccades, which have been linked to attentional modulation in V4 (Lowet et al., 2018). We computed information transmission separately for trials without microsaccades (Figure 4—figure supplement 4). In the population average of the two calculations for each session, we observed significant information transmission during the N2pc. Hence, microsaccade production was not responsible for the observed information theoretic associations between signals. The outcomes of these control analyses engender more confidence that the current dipole in V4 generated by the L2/3 CSD sink and the L5/6 CSD source contributes to the N2pc measured in the extracortical electric field.
Columnar feature selectivity influences contribution to N2pc
Given the columnar organization of color tuning of V4 neurons (Figure 5A; Roe et al., 2012; Zeki, 1973; Zeki, 1980; Tootell et al., 2004; Conway and Tsao, 2009; Kotake et al., 2009), we investigated the association between the N2pc and the CSD in columns with different color preferences. To quantify color selectivity through depth, we computed the response ratio between red and green stimuli (Figure 5B). Responses were measured as power in the gamma range (30–150 Hz) because this signal reflects local circuit interactions (Ray and Maunsell, 2011) and feature selectivity in visual cortex (Berens et al., 2008) and is more reliably measured than spiking activity across all LMA contacts. This analysis collapses across differences in color tuning across layers, so although the interlaminar specificity of gamma activity is not fully understood, recent work indicates that laminar gamma power can reliably reflect feature selectivity in a spatially specific fashion (Westerberg et al., 2021b).
To identify columns with significant selectivity for either red or green, we performed Wilcoxon signed rank tests between the distribution of ratios in each column against bootstrapped null distributions. Each bootstrapped null distribution contained 15 randomly selected ratios from the full dataset (450 experimental values) from which 1000 distributions were generated. The bootstrapped distributions represent the range of possible values observed across V4, but do not capture any difference in the homogeneity of feature selectivity within a column.
We found that more than half of V4 columns show selectivity for red or green stimuli (monkey Ca 12/21 [57.1%], He 5/9 [55.6%]). We computed the information transmission of target position for each color tuned column separately for trials when the preferred or the non-preferred color was in the column’s population RF. Across sessions with different target and distractor colors, we observed no difference in the amplitude of the extracortical signal during the N2pc (paired sample t(16) = 0.40, p = 0.69) (Figure 5C) nor the laminar CSD (L2/3: t(16) = –0.85, p = 0.41; L4: t(16) = 0.75, p = 0.46; L5/6: t(16) = 0.36, p = 0.72) (Figure 5D). However, information transmission during the N2pc was greater when a preferred rather than a non-preferred color was in the RF (Figure 5G). This difference was significant in L2/3 and L5/6 but not in L4 (t test across time with at least 10 ms having p < 0.05) and is evident in single sessions (Figure 5—figure supplement 1).
We investigated whether the magnitude of information transmission varied with degree of color preference. In session-wise correlations of the difference in information transmission between preferred and non-preferred colors at the time of peak information transmission (160–180 ms) as a function of columnar color selectivity index (CCSI), we found a significant relationship for L2/3 (Spearman’s R = 0.50, p = 0.005) and L5/6 (R = 0.51, p = 0.004) but not L4 (Figure 5H).
We also tested whether feature selective columns, on average, transmitted more information than their non-feature-selective counterparts. We found that feature selective columns, in all laminar compartments, transmitted significantly more information (Figure 5I) (two-sample t test: L2/3, p = 0.044; L4, p = 0.023; L5/6, p = 0.009). As such, we wanted to determine if this was due to a lack of attentional modulation in the non-selective columns. This was not the case, we observed that non-selective columns were modulated with attention. Attentional modulation was observed in both the CSD in L2/3 and L5/6 (one-sample t test: L2/3: t(64) = –6.01, p = 9.8e–8; L4: t(64) = –0.18, p = 0.86; L5/6: t(64) = 5.24, p = 1.9e–6) as well as across all layers in the population spiking activity (one-sample t test: L2/3: t(64) = 8.00, p = 3.7e–11; L4: t(64) = 9.66, p = 4.1e–14; L5/6: t(64) = 7.58, p = 1.8e–10) during the N2pc interval (averaged 150–190 ms following array onset) (Figure 5—figure supplement 2).
Importantly, we tested whether the N2pc varied across sessions with or without color-selective columns sampled. We found no difference between N2pc polarization (150–190 ms after the array) between sessions with (n = 17) or without (n = 13) sampling of color selective columns (two sample t test: t(28) = –0.75, p = 0.46). This invariance is expected because extracortical EEG spatially integrates signals from multiple cortical columns.
Translaminar currents in V4 recapitulate the N2pc
CSD is computed by differentiating between LFPs to eliminate volume-conducted signals that do not arise from local circuit activity. Using an inverse procedure (i.e., summing the CSD), it is possible to estimate the LFP without contamination by volume-conducted activity (Nicholson and Llinas, 1971; Kajikawa and Schroeder, 2011). We used this approach to compute an estimated extracortical ERP. Specifically, we computed the sum of currents produced by a cortical column to estimate the extracortical signal at a position directly above. The resultant potential (ERPcal) distinguished the target from a distractor in the RF throughout the N2pc (Figure 6). In other words, the summed potential generated by currents along V4 columns differentiates between attention conditions simultaneous with the extracortically measured attention-associated signal. Note that the shape of the observed extracortical ERP (ERPobs) differs from the estimated extracortical ERPcal. This is expected because the ERPobs reflects several more variables such as volume-conducted contributions of nearby columns as well as the filtering and attenuating effects of the tissue and cranium above the gray matter (Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006). Moreover, the ERPcal does not reflect the potential contributions of other visual areas. Given these expected differences, it is remarkable how well the difference in ERPcal predicts the timing of the attention-associated electric field.
Discussion
Bioelectric potentials have practical and clinical applications when their generators are known. For example, the electrocardiogram is useful in medicine because the physiological process associated with each phase of polarization is understood. Likewise, the electroretinogram is useful because the cell layers associated with each polarization are understood. In contrast, human ERP components indexing cognitive operations will have limited and only fortuitous utility until their neural generators are known.
The ERP indices of attention such as the N2pc or Pd are commonly used to assess the deployment of attention in human participants, but can also be observed in macaque monkeys, enabling systematic concurrent EEG and intracranial neurophysiological recordings. Our objective was to identify the neural generator of the attention-associated electric fields that comprise ERPs like the N2pc. Using inverse modeling of cranial surface EEG and laminar resolved CSD in a cortical area, we demonstrate that translaminar synaptic currents in visual cortical area V4 contribute to the generation of attention-associated electric fields during visual search. The dipole resulting in this electric field stemmed from layer-specific interactions in extragranular (top-down recipient) cortical layers. Unexpectedly, we discovered that the contribution of a cortical column to the overlying electric field depended on whether the visual feature in the RF matched the selectivity of the column – an important consideration in the mechanism producing EEG potentials that may not be observable through the macroscopic EEG signal alone.
The attention-associated electric field measured in our task is most likely representative of the commonly measured N2pc component of the EEG ERP. Given our findings regarding the functional architecture comprising attention-associated electric fields, it is conceivable that the N2pc arises from multiple, anatomically distinct cortical areas. That is, given the ubiquity of columnar architecture in sensory cortex and the specificity of visual feature representations to different cortical areas, electric dipoles formed across visual cortical layers could come about across multiple visual cortical areas with the relative contribution of each depending on the feature being attended to. This realization could help reconcile conflicting interpretations of the cognitive states and operations that are supposed to be indexed by the N2pc (Eimer, 1996; Kiss et al., 2008; Pagano and Mazza, 2012; Foster et al., 2020). It may also help account for variability in the N2pc as a function of attentional target presence in the lower versus upper visual hemifield (Luck et al., 1997b) given the positioning of retinotopic representations along the cortical surface (Gattass et al., 1988) – a potential focus for future study. Moreover, contributions from areas other than V4 are plausible because previous neurophysiological studies in macaques demonstrate attentional selection signals during visual search in the temporal (e.g., Sato, 1988), parietal (e.g., Bisley and Mirpour, 2019), and frontal (e.g., Thompson et al., 2005; Zhou and Desimone, 2011) lobes. Of particular note, neuroimaging studies in humans indicate a contribution to the N2pc from posterior parietal cortex (Hopf et al., 2000). In the same vein, FEF neurons locate the target among distractors as early as, or even before, the N2pc arises (Cohen et al., 2009; Purcell et al., 2013). Given the interconnectivity of FEF and V4 (Schall et al., 1995; Ungerleider et al., 2007; Gregoriou et al., 2012; Ninomiya et al., 2012), the frontal lobe could thus be the functional origin of an attentional selection signal communicated to V4 and other posterior areas (Armstrong and Moore, 2007; Ekstrom et al., 2009; Gregoriou et al., 2009; Gregoriou et al., 2012; Marshall et al., 2015; Popov et al., 2017), which in turn generate the N2pc (Westerberg and Schall, 2021a).
Our discovery that dipoles established by synaptic currents in visual cortical columns underlie the generation of the attention-associated electric fields is consistent with the observation that ERP components such as the N2pc are largest over the occipital lobe in humans (Luck and Hillyard, 1990; Luck and Hillyard, 1994) and macaques (Woodman et al., 2007), which is also observed with MEG (Hopf et al., 2000). Our investigation to identify interlaminar interactions producing the N2pc has offered unexpected insights into the underlying neural circuitry. Visual cortical area V4 contains a functional map of hue along its surface (Tanigawa et al., 2010) with individual columns comprising the map preferring the same color (Zeki, 1973; Zeki, 1980; Tootell et al., 2004; Conway and Tsao, 2009; Kotake et al., 2009; Westerberg et al., 2021b). We replicated the finding of columns specified by color-feature selectivity and discovered that the contribution of a column to the global electric field was greater for the preferred feature. Specifically, columns that preferred green (or red) contributed more to the electric field when the item in the RF was green (or red) rather than red (or green). This circuitry likely supports the decoding of visual features like color from EEG (Sandhaeger et al., 2019; Sutterer et al., 2021). The biophysical implications of this unexpected finding are illustrated in Figure 7, which portrays how an attention-associated electric field like the N2pc can arise from mosaics of different cortical columns. While all columns with the attended target in their RF contribute to the N2pc, columns with RF enclosing a target with preferred features establishes stronger dipoles than do columns representing other features or visual field locations. If target position or feature change, then other columns contribute the strongest dipoles.
However, these findings leave several unanswered questions. First, the details of the attentional mechanism enacted in the cortical microcircuit and in turn manifested in the EEG remain unknown. In this study, the task design required both spatial and feature-based attention. Existing models of attention put forward hypotheses for the findings reported here. For example, multiplicative gain would predict a larger increase in response for preferred versus non-preferred stimuli when attended in the RF (e.g., McAdams and Maunsell, 1999). Alternatively, feature similarity gain would predict that regardless of what is in the RF, red-preferring columns would increase activity and green-preferring would decrease activity when attending red and vice versa (e.g., Martinez-Trujillo and Treue, 2005). Further work should be undertaken to disentangle the contributions of spatial and feature attention to this attention-associated field generation. Second, we do not know whether the spatial shifts in the voltage distribution entailed by the mosaic hypothesis can be resolved on the human scalp due to smearing of the signals as they propagate through the skull and scalp (Nunez and Srinivasan, 2006). However, given decoding of features from EEG can be achieved (Sandhaeger et al., 2019; Sutterer et al., 2021), that would suggest some spatial information is preserved. Additionally, we do not know if this observation generalizes to other cortical areas or other ERP components. However, the discovery has this general implication: A given ERP can arise from qualitatively different neural circuit configurations. This implication entails specific limits on the nature of mechanistic inferences available from ERP measures.
Other aspects of the data deserve further consideration. First, our information theoretic analyses, while yielding clear results through rigorous means, produced values that were of noticeably smaller magnitude than what has been reported in previous studies (e.g., Optican and Richmond, 1987; Reich et al., 2001; Timme and Lapish, 2018). We are not concerned about this difference because no previous study has performed these measures on the relationship between intracortical synaptic currents and extracortical electric fields, so we have no strong prior about the magnitude of information theoretic results to expect. Also, the previous information theoretic analyses have been applied most commonly to measure relationships between pairs of single units, but we are comparing mesoscopic currents with macroscopic EEG, which is likely comprised of the activity of many cortical columns beyond that being concurrently measured intracortically.
Second, the polarity of the N2pc measured concurrently with intracortical laminar activity was opposite what has been previously described in macaque monkeys (Woodman et al., 2007). We believe this is an unfortunate consequence of differences in the referencing arrangements between the original and the present study. The previous work sampled EEG from an electrical lead embedded in the outer skull referenced to either linked ears or a frontal, extracranial electrode. We sampled EEG from LMA contact(s) outside the skull referenced to a rod supporting the LMA, which extended into the brain. As such, imbalanced measurement of the electric fields across the putative generator could lead to the inverted ERP polarity. We should also acknowledge possible differences caused by the presence of the craniotomy over the lunate gyrus. This curiosity can be resolved by sampling EEG from the cranial surface before and after a craniotomy with the alternative referencing arrangements. Regardless of the explanation, though, our findings of a strong association between V4 laminar substrates and the N2pc do not depend on the EEG polarization.
As a final note, it is important to consider what comes next for this program of research. Two avenues seem promising. In this study we are limited in that we only observed relationships between otherwise unaltered signals. While causal manipulations to neural circuits in cognitive tasks come with their own limitations (e.g., you are no longer observing the normal functioning system and consequent behavior), much could be gleaned about relative contributions from direct stimulation or inactivation of the putative circuitry generating these electric fields. In a similar vein, a biophysical modeling approach will yield more information on the relationships of attention-associated signals across scales. That is, by now knowing something about the circuit and mechanism yielding these attention-associated electric fields, we are able to use biophysically plausible computational models to gain further insight through simulations. Both approaches seem well suited to build on the findings detailed in this study. Ultimately the goal through these means and beyond should be to bridge the gap between what we know of the neurophysiology of attention at the microscopic scale to human attention-associated signals such as the N2pc.
Materials and methods
Animal care
Request a detailed protocolProcedures were in accordance with National Institutes of Health Guidelines, Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and approved by the Vanderbilt Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee following United States Department of Agriculture and Public Health Services policies. Animals were socially housed. Animals were on a 12 hr light-dark cycle and all experimental procedures were conducted in the daytime. Each monkey received nutrient-rich, primate-specific food pellets twice a day. Fresh produce and other forms of environmental enrichment were given at least five times a week.
Surgical procedures
Request a detailed protocolTwo male macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta monkey Z, 12.5 kg; Macaca radiata monkey P, 9 kg) were implanted with head posts and skull-embedded EEG arrays using previously described techniques (Woodman et al., 2007). One monkey (monkey P) was implanted with a subconjunctive eye coil. Two male macaque monkeys (Macaca radiata; monkey Ca, 7.5 kg; He, 7.3 kg) were implanted with head posts and MR compatible recording chambers with craniotomy over V4. Anesthetic induction was performed with ketamine (10 mg/kg). Monkeys were then catheterized and intubated. Surgeries were conducted aseptically with animals under O2, isoflurane (1–5%) anesthesia. EKG, temperature, and respiration were monitored. Postoperative antibiotics and analgesics were administered. Further detail is documented elsewhere (Woodman et al., 2007; Westerberg et al., 2020a; Westerberg et al., 2020b).
Magnetic resonance imaging
Request a detailed protocolAnesthetized animals were placed in a 3 T MRI scanner. T1-weighted three-dimensional (3D) MPRAGE scans were acquired with a 32-channel head coil equipped for SENSE imaging. Images were acquired using 0.5 mm isotropic voxel resolution with parameters: repetition 5 s, echo 2.5 ms, flip angle 7 degrees.
Visual search tasks
Request a detailed protocolMonkeys performed a color pop-out (monkeys Ca, He, and Z) or T/L (monkey P) search. Search arrays were presented on a CRT monitor at 60 Hz, at 57 cm distance. Stimulus generation and timing were done with TEMPO (Reflective Computing). Event times were assessed with a photodiode on the CRT. We used isoluminant red and green disks on a gray background (pop-out) or uniform gray T’s and L’s on a black background (T/L). Target feature varied within session for monkeys Ca, He, and Z. Monkey P identified the same target on any given session (T or L) but changed specific targets session to session. Trials were initiated by fixating within 1 (monkeys Ca and He) or 2 (monkeys P and Z) degrees of visual angle (dva) of a fixation dot. Time between fixation and array onset was at least 500 ms (monkey P: 500–1000 ms; Z: 500 ms; Ca and He: 750–1250 ms). For monkeys experiencing a range of fixation periods (monkeys Ca, He, P), a nonaging foreperiod function was used to determine the fixation period on a trial-by-trial basis. Arrays comprised of six (monkeys Ca and He) or eight (monkeys P and Z) items. Monkeys P and Z experienced invariable array eccentricity (10 dva) and item size (monkey P: 1.3 × 1.3 dva; Z: 1 × 1 dva). Two items were positioned on the vertical meridian, two on the horizontal, and the four remaining items equally spaced between. Monkeys Ca and He viewed items where size scaled with eccentricity at 0.3 dva per 1 dva eccentricity so that they were smaller than the average V4 RF (Freeman and Simoncelli, 2011). The angular position of items relative to fixation varied session to session so that one item was positioned at the center of the RF. Items were equally spaced relative to each other and located at the same eccentricity. Each trial, one array item was different from the others. Monkeys saccaded to the oddball within 1 (monkeys Ca and He) or 2 s (monkeys P and Z) and maintained fixation within 2–5 dva of the target for more than 400 ms (monkeys Ca, He, and Z: 500 ms; monkey P: 400–800 ms). Note that monkeys Ca, P, and Z were trained to versions of their respective search tasks that included catch trials where no target was present and they were tasked to remain fixating. Monkey He did not experience catch trials. Juice reward was administered following successful completion of the trial. The target item had an equal probability of being located at any of the six or eight locations. Eye movements were monitored at 1 kHz or 250 Hz using a corneal reflection system (monkeys Ca, He, and Z) or a scleral search coil (monkey P), respectively. Microsaccades were detected using an automatic algorithm (Otero-Millan et al., 2014). If the monkey failed to saccade to the target, they experienced a timeout (1–5 s).
10-20 EEG recordings
Request a detailed protocolTwo monkeys with intact skulls (i.e., lacking craniotomies) were implanted with an array of electrodes approximating the human 10–20 system locations (monkey P: FpFz, C3, C4, P3, P4, OL, OR, Oz; monkey Z: FpFz, Fpz, F3, F4, FCz, Cz, C3, C4, Pz, P5, P6, POz, O1, O2, Oz) (Jasper, 1958). Referencing was done using either the FpFz electrode (monkey P) or through linked ears (Z). The impedance of the individual electrodes was confirmed to be between 2 and 5 kOhm at 30 Hz, resembling electrodes used for human EEG. EEG was recorded using a Multichannel Acquisition Processor (Plexon) at 1 kHz and filtered between 0.7 and 170 Hz. Data was aligned to array onset and baseline corrected by subtracting the average activity during the 50 ms preceding the array onset from all timepoints. Data was clipped 20 ms prior to saccade to eliminate eye movement artifacts.
Simultaneous laminar V4 and extracortical recordings
Request a detailed protocolThe extracortical electric fields and laminar V4 neurophysiology were acquired at 24 kHz using a PZ5 and RZ2 (Tucker-Davis). Electric signals between 0.1 Hz and 12 kHz were observable with this system. V4 data was collected from two monkeys (monkey Ca: left hemisphere; He: right) across 30 sessions (monkey Ca: 21; monkey He: 9) using 32-channel linear electrode arrays with 0.1 mm interelectrode spacing (Plexon) introduced through the intact dura mater each session. Recordings were conducted with the electrode in a tube-grounded, reference-grounded configuration which grounds the stainless-steel support tube of the electrode and grounds the reference of the headstage. Arrays spanned layers of V4 with a subset of electrode contacts deliberately left outside of cortex. The extracortical electric field was derived from the most superficial electrode outside the brain (above the dura mater) using the same tube-grounded, referenced-grounded configuration and filtered between 1 and 100 Hz. CSD was computed from the raw signal by first extracting the LFP (signal filtered between 1 and 100 Hz, identical to the extracortical signal) and then taking the second spatial derivative along electrodes (Nicholson and Freeman, 1975; Schroeder et al., 1998; Mehta et al., 2000; Westerberg et al., 2019) and converting voltage to current (Logothetis et al., 2007). We computed the CSD by taking the second spatial derivative of the LFP:
where x is the extracellular voltage at time t measured at an electrode contact at depth d and z is the interelectrode distance and σ is conductivity. Both EEG and CSD were baseline corrected at the trial level by subtracting the average activation during the 300 ms preceding array onset from the response at all timepoints. Extracortical electric field potentials and CSD profiles were clipped 10 ms prior to saccade at the trial level to eliminate the influence of eye movements.
Population spiking was measured and analyzed to supplement primary LFP and CSD results. Multiunit activity was derived through well-documented means (Legatt et al., 1980) and has been demonstrated to be effective across multiple brain areas (Logothetis et al., 2001; Roelfsema et al., 2004; Self et al., 2013; Shapcott et al., 2016; Tovar et al., 2020; Westerberg et al., 2020a; Xing et al., 2009). The broadband neural signal was lowpass filtered at 3 kHz, highpass filtered at 300 Hz, full-wave rectified, and lastly, lowpass filtered at 150 Hz. This signal reliably reflect neural population dynamics (Trautmann et al., 2019). Additionally, multiunit activity in V4 has been shown to reliably reflect attentional modulation (Mehta et al., 2000; Nandy et al., 2017).
Laminar alignment
Request a detailed protocolOrthogonal array penetrations were confirmed online through a reverse-correlation RF mapping procedure (Nandy et al., 2017; Westerberg et al., 2019; Cox et al., 2019a; Cox et al., 2019b; Dougherty et al., 2019; Figure 2—figure supplement 1). RFs were found to represent portions of visual space consistent with previous reports of V4 (Gattass et al., 1988). Positions of recording sites relative to V4 layers were determined using CSD (Schroeder et al., 1998; Nandy et al., 2017). Current sinks following visual stimulation first appear in the granular input layers of cortex, then ascend to the supragranular compartment. Previously described observations of laminar V4 CSD include a sink in the infragranular layers following the ascent to the supragranular layers (Nandy et al., 2017), an observation we do not observe in our data. This is likely because we used task-evoked CSD for alignment with stimulus presentation persisting throughout the measurement interval whereas the descending sink observation was found with very brief stimulus presentations. It is likely that the strength of the persistent supragranular sink is masking the previously reported infragranular sink (Mitzdorf, 1985). We computed CSD and identified the granular input sink session-wise. Sessions were aligned by this input sink. ‘L4’ refers to granular input layer, ‘L2/3’ – supragranular layers, and ‘L5/6’ – infragranular layers. Each laminar compartment was assigned the same number of recording sites to alleviate biases during analysis.
Inverse modeling
Request a detailed protocolInverse modeling of 10–20 EEG recordings was performed in CURRY 8 (Compumedics Neuroscan). 3D head reconstruction was created for each monkey (P and Z) using the boundary element method (Hämäläinen and Sarvas, 1989). This method takes into account individual monkey’s surface morphologies to create models of cortex surface, inner and outer skull, and skin boundaries. This model was used in conjunction with EEG to compute a voltage distribution over the cortical surface. We calculated the current density with sLORETA, which calculates a minimum norm least squares that divides current by the size of its associated error bar, yielding F scores of activation. sLORETA produces blurred but accurate localizations of point sources (Pascual-Marqui, 2002). Other algorithms such as minimum norm and SWARM were modeled as well, with agreement between models sufficient not to change any conclusions.
Information theory analyses
Request a detailed protocolInformation theory (Shannon, 1948) analyses were chosen for several reasons. First, information theory analyses yield results in terms of ‘bits’ which can be used to directly compare effect sizes across measurement methods (e.g., CSD, extracortical signal, and array composition [directed spatial attention]). Next, these analyses are inherently multivariate and able to capture linear and nonlinear relationships. Furthermore, information theory is model independent and does not necessitate a specific hypothetical structure in order to detect relationships between signals. This combination allows us to detect relationships between the extracortical signal and CSD signal that might not be linear and therefore would not be captured by linear models or correlation analyses. We chose to measure pairwise mutual information and information transmission to gauge the relationships between our three ‘signals’ (e.g., extracortical, CSD, and array composition [directed spatial attention]). Mutual information is the reduction in uncertainty in one variable afforded by another known variable. That is, mutual information is greater when you know the state of one variable covaries with the state of the other variable. If the two variables do not correspond well, mutual information is low. Therefore, the reduction in uncertainty is formalized as ‘information’ which is relayed in bits. Mathematically, mutual information is captured by the following equation (Cover and Thomas, 2006; Beer and Williams, 2015):
where H(X) and H(X|Y) are the entropy X and X given Y, respectively. Entropy for a signal (S) is computed by:
where p(s) is the probability distribution for signal s and i is the signal state. Therefore, mutual information can be computed probabilistically by:
where p(x), p(y) are the probability distributions for X and Y, and p(x,y) is the joint probability distribution of X and Y across signal states i and j for X and Y, respectively.
While mutual information describes the relationship between the two signals (for our purpose: CSD and the extracortical signal, CSD and directed spatial attention, or the extracortical signal and directed spatial attention), it does not allow for the evaluation of two signals regarding a third (e.g., CSD and the extracortical signal regarding directed spatial attention). For analyses where we want to understand information regarding the allocation of directed attention from the synaptic currents in V4 to the extracortical signal, we use a modified equation rooted in the same entropy/mutual information principles. In computing information transmission, we are interested in the information about X (directed spatial attention), transferred from Y (CSD in V4) to Z (extracortical signal) formalized as:
where past and future describe the timepoints when the data is taken from. The information transmission (IT) is taken as the difference between two minimum information calculations. The minimum information (Imin) is computed regarding the combination of the individual signals (S1 and S2) at the specified intervals as:
where p(x) is the probability distribution for signal X and x are the possible states of X. By taking into account different timepoints for the signals, we can interpret this computation as the information about X (directed spatial attention) shared by Ypast (e.g., earlier CSD in V4) and Zfuture (e.g., later extracortical signal) that was not already in Zpast (e.g., earlier extracortical signal).
Above information theory analyses were performed using the Neuroscience Information Theory Toolbox (Timme and Lapish, 2018). Pairwise mutual information and information transmission were computed at each timepoint across trials for each session using default parameters. Five uniform count bins were used for data binning; 10 ms was used for time lag for information transmission. Only correct trials were included. Information theory measures were computed for each millisecond for the entire interval displayed in each analyses’ respective figure panel. CSD for each laminar compartment was computed by taking the average activity of five sites at the trial level included in each laminar compartment. For mutual information between target position and the extracortical signal, target position was binary where target was either contra- or ipsilaterally presented. For computations within V4, target position was binary where target was either in the RF or positioned opposite the RF; 5000 Monte Carlo simulations were used to generate a distribution of null model values which experimental values were compared to (α = 0.05).
Feature selectivity
Request a detailed protocolFor each recording site within a column, gamma power (30–150 Hz) (Maier et al., 2010) responses were computed when either a red distractor was presented to the RF of the column or when a green distractor was present to the RF. Responses were taken as the average activation 75–200 ms following array onset. An index was computed from these responses by subtracting the 2 and dividing by their sum (CSI). Values were therefore bounded between –1 and 1 where larger magnitude indicates greater selectivity for green (toward –1) or red (toward 1). CCSI was computed as the average of CSIs along the entire column. We performed Wilcoxon signed rank tests on the distribution of CSIs across the recording sites of a given cortical column to determine whether a column was significantly color selective (α = 0.05). The selective columns were included in feature selectivity analyses where the preferred color and non-preferred color were defined as the color that elicited greater and lesser responses, respectively.
Estimating field potential from CSD
Request a detailed protocolWe calculated the ERP at arbitrary positions from the measured laminar CSD (ERPcal) using a previously described model (Nicholson and Llinas, 1971; Kajikawa and Schroeder, 2011).
where ERPcal at depth i (dt) for each timepoint (t) is taken as the sum of CSD at depths j (dj) for each timepoint divided by the Euclidean distance to account for the diminishing impact of local currents on more distant field potentials. The factor A acts only as a scaling factor and we cannot accurately estimate the magnitude of the 1D CSD-derived waveform, so we eliminate this parameter from the calculation. This omission is consistent with previous reports (Kajikawa and Schroeder, 2011) and limits our comparisons of observed ERP and ERPcal to only shape. However, magnitude differences can be observed between conditions for ERPobs and ERPcal, independently. Also, for our purposes, we set h to 0 as we assume that our observed CSD and the calculated ERP are in the same vertical plane.
Data availability
Data supporting the findings documented in this study are freely available online through Dryad at https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.djh9w0w15.
-
Dryad Digital RepositoryData from: Laminar microcircuitry of visual cortex producing attention-associated electric fields.https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.djh9w0w15
References
-
Information processing and dynamics in minimally cognitive agentsCognitive Science 39:1–38.https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12142
-
The neural instantiation of a priority mapCurrent Opinion in Psychology 29:108–112.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.01.002
-
On the origin of event-related potentials indexing covert attentional selection during visual searchJournal of Neurophysiology 102:2375–2386.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00680.2009
-
Where Does EEG Come From and What Does It Mean?Trends in Neurosciences 40:208–218.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2017.02.004
-
Prefrontal Control of Visual DistractionCurrent Biology 28:414–420.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.023
-
Spiking Suppression Precedes Cued Attentional Enhancement of Neural Responses in Primary Visual CortexCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 29:77–90.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx305
-
Temporal dynamics of binocular integration in primary visual cortexJournal of Vision 19:1–13.https://doi.org/10.1167/19.12.13
-
Binocular Modulation of Monocular V1 NeuronsCurrent Biology 29:381–391.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.12.004
-
A Canonical Microcircuit for NeocortexNeural Computation 1:480–488.https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.1989.1.4.480
-
A functional microcircuit for cat visual cortexThe Journal of Physiology 440:735–769.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018733
-
The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivityElectroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 99:225–234.https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9
-
Modulation of the contrast response function by electrical microstimulation of the macaque frontal eye fieldThe Journal of Neuroscience 29:10683–10694.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0673-09.2009
-
Selective modulation of cortical state during spatial attentionScience (New York, N.Y.) 354:1140–1144.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aag1420
-
Covert Spatial Attention Speeds Target IndividuationThe Journal of Neuroscience 40:2717–2726.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2962-19.2020
-
Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attentionScience (New York, N.Y.) 291:1560–1563.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1055465
-
Visuotopic organization and extent of V3 and V4 of the macaqueThe Journal of Neuroscience 8:1831–1845.
-
Microcircuitry of agranular frontal cortex: testing the generality of the canonical cortical microcircuitThe Journal of Neuroscience 34:5355–5369.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5127-13.2014
-
Review on solving the inverse problem in EEG source analysisJournal of Neuroengineering and Rehabilitation 5:25.https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-5-25
-
High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attentionScience (New York, N.Y.) 324:1207–1210.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1171402
-
Realistic conductivity geometry model of the human head for interpretation of neuromagnetic dataIEEE Transactions on Bio-Medical Engineering 36:165–171.https://doi.org/10.1109/10.16463
-
Examining local network processing using multi-contact laminar electrode recordingJournal of Visualized Experiments 8:2806.https://doi.org/10.3791/2806
-
Electrophysiological indices of target and distractor processing in visual searchJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21:760–775.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21039
-
Neural sources of focused attention in visual searchCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 10:1233–1241.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/10.12.1233
-
The ten-twenty electrode system of the international federationElectroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 10:370–375.
-
Organization of color-selective neurons in macaque visual area V4Journal of Neurophysiology 102:15–27.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90624.2008
-
Electrophysiological evidence for parallel and serial processing during visual searchPerception & Psychophysics 48:603–617.https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03211606
-
Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortexJournal of Neurophysiology 77:24–42.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1997.77.1.24
-
Distinct superficial and deep laminar domains of activity in the visual cortex during rest and stimulationFrontiers in Systems Neuroscience 4:31.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2010.00031
-
BookThe feature similarity gain model of attention: unifying multiplicative effects of spatial and feature-based attentionIn: Itti L, Rees G, Tsotsos JK, editors. Neurobiology of Attention. Academic Press. pp. 300–304.https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012375731-9/50053-7
-
Effects of attention on orientation-tuning functions of single neurons in macaque cortical area V4The Journal of Neuroscience 19:431–441.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00431.1999
-
Intermodal selective attention in monkeys. II: physiological mechanisms of modulationCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 10:359–370.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/10.4.359
-
EEG source imagingClinical Neurophysiology 115:2195–2222.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2004.06.001
-
Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortexScience (New York, N.Y.) 229:782–784.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.4023713
-
The columnar organization of the neocortexBrain: A Journal of Neurology 120 ( Pt 4):701–722.https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.4.701
-
Effects of neural synchrony on surface EEGCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 24:1045–1053.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs389
-
Theory of current source-density analysis and determination of conductivity tensor for anuran cerebellumJournal of Neurophysiology 38:356–368.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1975.38.2.356
-
Segregated pathways carrying frontally derived top-down signals to visual areas MT and V4 in macaquesThe Journal of Neuroscience 32:6851–6858.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6295-11.2012
-
BookElectric Fields of the Brain: The Neurophysics of EEGOxford University Press.https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195050387.001.0001
-
Unsupervised clustering method to detect microsaccadesJournal of Vision 14:18.https://doi.org/10.1167/14.2.18
-
Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical detailsMethods and Findings in Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology 24 Suppl D:5–12.
-
FEF-Controlled Alpha Delay Activity Precedes Stimulus-Induced Gamma-Band Activity in Visual CortexThe Journal of Neuroscience 37:4117–4127.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3015-16.2017
-
Formal and attribute-specific information in primary visual cortexJournal of Neurophysiology 85:305–318.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2001.85.1.305
-
Competitive mechanisms subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4The Journal of Neuroscience 19:1736–1753.
-
Effects of attention and stimulus interaction on visual responses of inferior temporal neurons in macaqueJournal of Neurophysiology 60:344–364.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1988.60.1.344
-
Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and segregation of processing streamsThe Journal of Neuroscience 15:4464–4487.
-
Simultaneous EEG and fMRI in the macaque monkey at 4.7 TeslaMagnetic Resonance Imaging 24:335–342.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mri.2005.12.024
-
Subcortical contributions to the surface-recorded flash-VEP in the awake macaqueElectroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 84:219–231.https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-5597(92)90003-t
-
A spatiotemporal profile of visual system activation revealed by current source density analysis in the awake macaqueCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 8:575–592.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/8.7.575
-
A Mathematical Theory of CommunicationBell System Technical Journal 27:379–423.https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x
-
Stimulus-dependent spiking relationships with the EEGJournal of Neurophysiology 114:1468–1482.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00427.2015
-
Decoding chromaticity and luminance from patterns of EEG activityPsychophysiology 58:e13779.https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13779
-
Functional organization for color and orientation in macaque V4Nature Neuroscience 13:1542–1548.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2676
-
Neuronal basis of covert spatial attention in the frontal eye fieldThe Journal of Neuroscience 25:9479–9487.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0741-05.2005
-
A Tutorial for Information Theory in NeuroscienceENeuro 5:ENEURO.0052-18.2018.https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0052-18.2018
-
Search for color “center(s)” in macaque visual cortexCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 14:353–363.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh001
-
Cortical Connections of Area V4 in the MacaqueCerebral Cortex 18:477–499.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm061
-
Performance Monitoring during Visual PrimingJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 32:515–526.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01499
-
Neural mechanism of priming in visual searchAttention, Perception & Psychophysics 83:587–602.https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02118-8
-
Spatial Spread of the Local Field Potential and its Laminar Variation in Visual CortexJournal of Neuroscience 29:11540–11549.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2573-09.2009
-
Colour coding in rhesus monkey prestriate cortexBrain Research 53:422–427.https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(73)90227-8
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Eye Institute (F31EY031293)
- Jacob A Westerberg
National Eye Institute (P30EY008126)
- Alexander Maier
- Geoffrey F Woodman
- Jeffrey D Schall
National Eye Institute (R01EY019882)
- Geoffrey F Woodman
- Jeffrey D Schall
National Eye Institute (R01EY008890)
- Jeffrey D Schall
National Eye Institute (R01EY027402)
- Alexander Maier
Office of the Director (S10OD021771)
- Alexander Maier
- Geoffrey F Woodman
- Jeffrey D Schall
National Eye Institute (T32EY007135)
- Jacob A Westerberg
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by NIH through the NEI (P30EY008126, R01EY019882, R01EY008890, R01EY027402) and the Office of the Director (S10OD021771). JAW was supported by fellowships from NEI (F31EY031293 and T32EY007135). The authors would like to thank I Haniff, M Feurtado, M Maddox, S Motorny, D Richardson, L Toy, B Williams, R Williams for technical support. The authors would like to thank B Purcell, P Weigand for collecting data, and S Errington, B Herrera, K Lowe, T Reppert, J Riera, A Sajad, E Sigworth for useful conversations regarding the work.
Ethics
Procedures were in accordance with National Institutes of Health Guidelines, Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and approved by the Vanderbilt Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol M1700067) following United States Department of Agriculture and Public Health Services policies.
Copyright
© 2022, Westerberg et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,277
- views
-
- 205
- downloads
-
- 15
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Gamma oscillations in brain activity (30–150 Hz) have been studied for over 80 years. Although in the past three decades significant progress has been made to try to understand their functional role, a definitive answer regarding their causal implication in perception, cognition, and behavior still lies ahead of us. Here, we first review the basic neural mechanisms that give rise to gamma oscillations and then focus on two main pillars of exploration. The first pillar examines the major theories regarding their functional role in information processing in the brain, also highlighting critical viewpoints. The second pillar reviews a novel research direction that proposes a therapeutic role for gamma oscillations, namely the gamma entrainment using sensory stimulation (GENUS). We extensively discuss both the positive findings and the issues regarding reproducibility of GENUS. Going beyond the functional and therapeutic role of gamma, we propose a third pillar of exploration, where gamma, generated endogenously by cortical circuits, is essential for maintenance of healthy circuit function. We propose that four classes of interneurons, namely those expressing parvalbumin (PV), vasointestinal peptide (VIP), somatostatin (SST), and nitric oxide synthase (NOS) take advantage of endogenous gamma to perform active vasomotor control that maintains homeostasis in the neuronal tissue. According to this hypothesis, which we call GAMER (GAmma MEdiated ciRcuit maintenance), gamma oscillations act as a ‘servicing’ rhythm that enables efficient translation of neural activity into vascular responses that are essential for optimal neurometabolic processes. GAMER is an extension of GENUS, where endogenous rather than entrained gamma plays a fundamental role. Finally, we propose several critical experiments to test the GAMER hypothesis.
-
- Medicine
- Neuroscience
The advent of midazolam holds profound implications for modern clinical practice. The hypnotic and sedative effects of midazolam afford it broad clinical applicability. However, the specific mechanisms underlying the modulation of altered consciousness by midazolam remain elusive. Herein, using pharmacology, optogenetics, chemogenetics, fiber photometry, and gene knockdown, this in vivo research revealed the role of locus coeruleus (LC)-ventrolateral preoptic nucleus noradrenergic neural circuit in regulating midazolam-induced altered consciousness. This effect was mediated by α1 adrenergic receptors. Moreover, gamma-aminobutyric acid receptor type A (GABAA-R) represents a mechanistically crucial binding site in the LC for midazolam. These findings will provide novel insights into the neural circuit mechanisms underlying the recovery of consciousness after midazolam administration and will help guide the timing of clinical dosing and propose effective intervention targets for timely recovery from midazolam-induced loss of consciousness.