Spatial signatures of anesthesia-induced burst-suppression differ between primates and rodents

  1. Nikoloz Sirmpilatze  Is a corresponding author
  2. Judith Mylius
  3. Michael Ortiz-Rios
  4. Jürgen Baudewig
  5. Jaakko Paasonen
  6. Daniel Golkowski
  7. Andreas Ranft
  8. Rüdiger Ilg
  9. Olli Gröhn
  10. Susann Boretius  Is a corresponding author
  1. Functional Imaging Laboratory, German Primate Center – Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Germany
  2. Georg-August University of Göttingen, Germany
  3. International Max Planck Research School for Neurosciences, Germany
  4. A.I.V. Institute for Molecular Sciences, University of Eastern Finland, Finland
  5. Department of Neurology, Klinikum Rechts der Isar der Technischen Universität München, Germany
  6. Department of Neurology, Heidelberg University Hospital, Germany
  7. Department of Anesthesiology and Intensive Care Medicine, Klinikum Rechts der Isar der Technischen Universität München, Germany
  8. Department of Neurology, Asklepios Stadtklinik Bad Tölz, Germany
  9. Leibniz Science Campus Primate Cognition, Germany

Peer review process

This article was accepted for publication as part of eLife's original publishing model.

History

  1. Version of Record published
  2. Accepted
  3. Received
  4. Preprint posted

Decision letter

  1. Ming Meng
    Reviewing Editor; South China Normal University, China
  2. Tamar R Makin
    Senior Editor; University College London, United Kingdom
  3. Shuntaro Sasai
    Reviewer; University of Wisconsin-Madison, United States
  4. Cornelius Faber
    Reviewer; University of Münster, Germany

Our editorial process produces two outputs: i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Spatial signatures of anesthesia-induced burst-suppression differ between primates and rodents" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Tamar Makin as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Shuntaro Sasai (Reviewer #2); Cornelius Faber (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential revisions:

While the reviewers were in general positive about the research and the paper, a number of issues were raised by Reviewer #1 and Reviewer #2. Below are the main points, which are considered essential revisions and the authors should address point-by-point.

1) As seen in the tSNR map for macaques and marmosets, the tSNR around the primary visual cortex was much weaker than other cortices. Moreover, in marmosets, the EPI slices did not cover the entire brain and actually left most of the V1 uncovered. It would be better to analyze and discuss how the tSNR differences affect the present findings. For example, the author may consider including the tSNR as covariance in their map analysis.

2) Strengthen validity of the results: look into the EEG signals around the sensory cortex (e.g., V1) to see whether the findings in fMRI could be confirmed.

3) Clarifications:

a) As seen in Figure 2—figure supplement 2, there was a significant anticorrelation with burst-suppression at the ventricular borders. It is unclear whether the authors have done physiological or white matter/CSF/global nuisance regression as most of the rest-fMRI studies did.

b) Three different concentrations of the anesthetic sevoflurane were chosen for human participants. The authors found that the high concentration (3.9-4.6%) induced burst-suppression much better than the other two lower concentrations as expected. However, in rats, almost all asymmetric PCs were found at an intermediate concentration (2%) of isoflurane less at the low (1.5%) or high (2.5%) concentration in Rat 1. At the same time, all fMRI runs from Rat 2 with a 1.3% concentration of isoflurane had a prominent asymmetric PC. That is, it seems that only the high concentration of isoflurane could not induce burst-suppression well in rats, which was opposite to those findings in humans. Authors may explain what reasons may cause such differences and whether such differences may affect the major findings in differences between primates and rodents.

c) Authors found that some sensory areas in primates are excluded from those highly synchronized during the burst suppression. Clarify if each voxel in such areas shows burst suppression-like activity that is not synchronized with others. If this is the case, burst suppression can still be a global phenomenon. Because that in-ROI averaged time-series were used, it remains possible that each voxel inside the ROI is not synchronized but the ROI average shows burst suppression. Similarly, since that burst suppression should be defined by the existence of burst and suppressed periods, why not simply use this definition to check whether each voxel shows such an intermittent activity to evaluate whether it is a global phenomenon or not. We understand that this might be difficult to examine using non-human fMRI data, however, voxelwise analysis of human fMRI data should be conducted.

d) Why is there no synchronization during the slow-wave states under light anesthesia? During the slow-wave sleep, it is shown that the entire cortical network is decomposed into a modular-like network structure. Is there synchronization inside each module while no synchrony between modules?

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.74813.sa1

Author response

Essential revisions:

While the reviewers were in general positive about the research and the paper, a number of issues were raised by Reviewer #1 and Reviewer #2. Below are the main points, which are considered essential revisions and the authors should address point-by-point.

1) As seen in the tSNR map for macaques and marmosets, the tSNR around the primary visual cortex was much weaker than other cortices. Moreover, in marmosets, the EPI slices did not cover the entire brain and actually left most of the V1 uncovered. It would be better to analyze and discuss how the tSNR differences affect the present findings. For example, the author may consider including the tSNR as covariance in their map analysis.

See response to Public Review Reviewer #1 major issue 1.

2) Strengthen validity of the results: look into the EEG signals around the sensory cortex (e.g., V1) to see whether the findings in fMRI could be confirmed.

See response to Public Review Reviewer #1 major issue 2.

3) Clarifications:

a) As seen in Figure 2—figure supplement 2, there was a significant anticorrelation with burst-suppression at the ventricular borders. It is unclear whether the authors have done physiological or white matter/CSF/global nuisance regression as most of the rest-fMRI studies did.

See response to Public Review Reviewer #1 major issue 3.

b) Three different concentrations of the anesthetic sevoflurane were chosen for human participants. The authors found that the high concentration (3.9-4.6%) induced burst-suppression much better than the other two lower concentrations as expected. However, in rats, almost all asymmetric PCs were found at an intermediate concentration (2%) of isoflurane less at the low (1.5%) or high (2.5%) concentration in Rat 1. At the same time, all fMRI runs from Rat 2 with a 1.3% concentration of isoflurane had a prominent asymmetric PC. That is, it seems that only the high concentration of isoflurane could not induce burst-suppression well in rats, which was opposite to those findings in humans. Authors may explain what reasons may cause such differences and whether such differences may affect the major findings in differences between primates and rodents.

See response to Public Review Reviewer #1 major issue 4.

c) Authors found that some sensory areas in primates are excluded from those highly synchronized during the burst suppression. Clarify if each voxel in such areas shows burst suppression-like activity that is not synchronized with others. If this is the case, burst suppression can still be a global phenomenon. Because that in-ROI averaged time-series were used, it remains possible that each voxel inside the ROI is not synchronized but the ROI average shows burst suppression. Similarly, since that burst suppression should be defined by the existence of burst and suppressed periods, why not simply use this definition to check whether each voxel shows such an intermittent activity to evaluate whether it is a global phenomenon or not. We understand that this might be difficult to examine using non-human fMRI data, however, voxelwise analysis of human fMRI data should be conducted.

See responses to Public Review Reviewer #2 issues 1–2.

d) Why is there no synchronization during the slow-wave states under light anesthesia? During the slow-wave sleep, it is shown that the entire cortical network is decomposed into a modular-like network structure. Is there synchronization inside each module while no synchrony between modules?

See response to Public Review Reviewer #2 issue 3.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.74813.sa2

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Nikoloz Sirmpilatze
  2. Judith Mylius
  3. Michael Ortiz-Rios
  4. Jürgen Baudewig
  5. Jaakko Paasonen
  6. Daniel Golkowski
  7. Andreas Ranft
  8. Rüdiger Ilg
  9. Olli Gröhn
  10. Susann Boretius
(2022)
Spatial signatures of anesthesia-induced burst-suppression differ between primates and rodents
eLife 11:e74813.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.74813

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.74813