Shared mechanisms of auditory and non-auditory vocal learning in the songbird brain
Abstract
Songbirds and humans share the ability to adaptively modify their vocalizations based on sensory feedback. Prior studies have focused primarily on the role that auditory feedback plays in shaping vocal output throughout life. In contrast, it is unclear how non-auditory information drives vocal plasticity. Here, we first used a reinforcement learning paradigm to establish that somatosensory feedback (cutaneous electrical stimulation) can drive vocal learning in adult songbirds. We then assessed the role of a songbird basal ganglia thalamocortical pathway critical to auditory vocal learning in this novel form of vocal plasticity. We found that both this circuit and its dopaminergic inputs are necessary for non-auditory vocal learning, demonstrating that this pathway is critical for guiding adaptive vocal changes based on both auditory and somatosensory signals. The ability of this circuit to use both auditory and somatosensory information to guide vocal learning may reflect a general principle for the neural systems that support vocal plasticity across species.
Data availability
Source data are provided for all main figures and relevant figure supplements (Figure 2b-f, Figure 2 - Figure Supplements 1-7, Figure 3b-e, Figure 3 - Figure Supplement 1, and Figure 4b-d). MATLAB code for generating these figures is also provided in the associated source code files. Data and source code have also been uploaded to a public data repository on figshare, in a project titled 'Shared mechanisms of auditory and non-auditory vocal learning in the songbird brain.'
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_4_Source_data_3.mat. figshare. Dataset.Figshare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183351.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_4_Source_data_1.mat. figshare. Dataset.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183354.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_3_Source_Code_3.m. figshare. Software.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183357.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_4_Source_Code_2.m. figshare. Software.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183360.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_3_Source_data_1.mat. figshare. Dataset.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183348.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_4_Source_Code_3.m. figshare. Software.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183339.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_4_Source_data_2.mat. figshare. Dataset.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183342.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_2_Source_data_3.mat. figshare. Dataset.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183345.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_3_Source_Code_2.m. figshare. Software.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183327.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_3_Source_data_2.mat. figshare. Dataset.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183330.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_2_Source_data_2.mat. figshare. Dataset.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183333.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_3_Source_Code_4.m. figshare. Software.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183336.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_2_Source_data_1.mat. figshare. Dataset.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183318.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_3_Source_data_3.mat. figshare. Dataset.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183321.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_4_Source_Code_1.m. figshare. Software.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183324.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_2_Source_code_3.m. figshare. Software.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183315.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_2_Source_code_4.m. figshare. Software.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183303.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_2_Source_Code_1.m. figshare. Software.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183306.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_2_Source_Code_2.m. figshare. Software.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183309.v1.
-
McGregor_et_al_Figure_3_Source_Code_1.m. figshare. Software.FigShare, 10.6084/m9.figshare.20183312.v1.
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institutes of Health (R01- EB022872)
- James N McGregor
- Abigail L Grassler
- Amanda Louise Jacob
- Samuel J Sober
National Institutes of Health (R01- NS084844)
- James N McGregor
- Abigail L Grassler
- Amanda Louise Jacob
- Samuel J Sober
National Institutes of Health (R01- NS099375)
- James N McGregor
- Abigail L Grassler
- Amanda Louise Jacob
- Samuel J Sober
Simons Foundation (Emory International Consortium on Motor Control)
- Samuel J Sober
Howard Hughes Medical Institute
- Paul I Jaffe
- Michael S Brainard
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: All experimental protocols were approved by the Emory University and UC San Francisco Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (protocol #201700359)
Copyright
© 2022, McGregor et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,436
- views
-
- 513
- downloads
-
- 16
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Detecting causal relations structures our perception of events in the world. Here, we determined for visual interactions whether generalized (i.e. feature-invariant) or specialized (i.e. feature-selective) visual routines underlie the perception of causality. To this end, we applied a visual adaptation protocol to assess the adaptability of specific features in classical launching events of simple geometric shapes. We asked observers to report whether they observed a launch or a pass in ambiguous test events (i.e. the overlap between two discs varied from trial to trial). After prolonged exposure to causal launch events (the adaptor) defined by a particular set of features (i.e. a particular motion direction, motion speed, or feature conjunction), observers were less likely to see causal launches in subsequent ambiguous test events than before adaptation. Crucially, adaptation was contingent on the causal impression in launches as demonstrated by a lack of adaptation in non-causal control events. We assessed whether this negative aftereffect transfers to test events with a new set of feature values that were not presented during adaptation. Processing in specialized (as opposed to generalized) visual routines predicts that the transfer of visual adaptation depends on the feature similarity of the adaptor and the test event. We show that the negative aftereffects do not transfer to unadapted launch directions but do transfer to launch events of different speeds. Finally, we used colored discs to assign distinct feature-based identities to the launching and the launched stimulus. We found that the adaptation transferred across colors if the test event had the same motion direction as the adaptor. In summary, visual adaptation allowed us to carve out a visual feature space underlying the perception of causality and revealed specialized visual routines that are tuned to a launch’s motion direction.
-
- Neuroscience
The classical diagnosis of Parkinsonism is based on motor symptoms that are the consequence of nigrostriatal pathway dysfunction and reduced dopaminergic output. However, a decade prior to the emergence of motor issues, patients frequently experience non-motor symptoms, such as a reduced sense of smell (hyposmia). The cellular and molecular bases for these early defects remain enigmatic. To explore this, we developed a new collection of five fruit fly models of familial Parkinsonism and conducted single-cell RNA sequencing on young brains of these models. Interestingly, cholinergic projection neurons are the most vulnerable cells, and genes associated with presynaptic function are the most deregulated. Additional single nucleus sequencing of three specific brain regions of Parkinson’s disease patients confirms these findings. Indeed, the disturbances lead to early synaptic dysfunction, notably affecting cholinergic olfactory projection neurons crucial for olfactory function in flies. Correcting these defects specifically in olfactory cholinergic interneurons in flies or inducing cholinergic signaling in Parkinson mutant human induced dopaminergic neurons in vitro using nicotine, both rescue age-dependent dopaminergic neuron decline. Hence, our research uncovers that one of the earliest indicators of disease in five different models of familial Parkinsonism is synaptic dysfunction in higher-order cholinergic projection neurons and this contributes to the development of hyposmia. Furthermore, the shared pathways of synaptic failure in these cholinergic neurons ultimately contribute to dopaminergic dysfunction later in life.