Abstract
Comparisons across adults with different sensory histories (blind vs. sighted) have uncovered effects of experience on human brain function. In people born blind visual cortices are responsive to non-visual tasks and show altered functional connectivity at rest. Since almost all research has been done with adults, little is known about the developmental origins of this plasticity. Are infant visual cortices initially functionally like those of sighted adults and blindness causes reorganization? Alternatively, do infants start like blind adults, with vision required to set up the sighted pattern? To distinguish between these possibilities, we compare resting state functional connectivity across blind (n = 30) and blindfolded sighted (n = 50) adults to a large cohort of sighted infants (Developing Human Connectome Project, n = 475). Remarkably, we find that infant secondary visual cortices functionally resemble those of blind more than sighted adults, consistent with the idea that visual experience is required to set up long-range functional connectivity. Primary visual cortices show a mixture of instructive effects of vision and reorganizing effects of blindness. Specifically, in sighted adults, visual cortices show stronger functional coupling with nonvisual sensory-motor networks (i.e., auditory, somatosensory/motor) than with higher-cognitive prefrontal cortices (PFC). In blind adults, visual cortices show stronger coupling with PFC. In infants, connectivity of secondary visual cortices is stronger with PFC, while V1 shows equal sensory-motor/PFC connectivity. In contrast, lateralization of occipital-to-frontal connectivity resembles the sighted adults at birth and is reorganized by blindness, possibly due to recruitment of occipital networks for lateralized cognitive functions, such as language.
Introduction
Studies of vision loss provide insight into how early life experience shapes cortical function and behavior. Visual cortices of adults born blind show enhanced responses during non-visual tasks, such as Braille reading by touch, spoken language comprehension and auditory spatial processing (Bedny et al., 2011; Burton et al., 2012; Collignon et al., 2011; Kanjlia et al., 2016, 2021; Lane et al., 2015; Masuda et al., 2021; N. Raz et al., 2005; Sadato et al., 1996). Occipital cortices of blind adults also show distinctive patterns of spontaneous neural activity and functional connectivity (resting state correlations) with non-visual networks (Abboud & Cohen, 2019; Bedny et al., 2011; Burton et al., 2014; Butt et al., 2013; Deen et al., 2015; Y. Liu et al., 2007; Striem-Amit et al., 2015; Watkins et al., 2012). The developmental origins of these function differences across blind and sighted people are not known, since almost all research is done with adults.
One possibility is that at birth infants start out in the ‘prepared’ sighted adult state and differences among sighted and blind adults reflects reorganization caused by blindness. However, it is also possible that some of the functional differences observed between blind and sighted adults reflect the lack of instructive effects of visual experience. In other words, infants start out similar to blind adults and vision ‘instructs’ the sighted adult pattern. To distinguish between these possibilities, we compare the functional connectivity profile of visual cortices of blind and sighted adults to a large cohort of sighted infants on average 2-weeks old (Developing Human Connectome Project, dHCP, n = 475). We used resting state data as a common measure of cortical function across these diverse populations.
Previous resting state studies comparing sighted infants to sighted adults have largely reported similarities, implying a ‘prepared’ sighted pattern (Barttfeld et al., 2018; Doria et al., 2010; Fransson et al., 2009; Gao et al., 2009; W. C. Liu et al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2019). However, these prior studies focused mostly on connectivity within large scale functional networks (e.g., visual areas are more correlated with other visual areas than with somatosensory networks), whereas differences between blind and sighted adults are observed in connectivity between the visual system and non-visual functional networks i.e., which non-visual networks are most correlated with the visual system differs across blind an sighted adults.
We examined the connectivity profile of three secondary visual areas on the lateral, dorsal and ventral occipital surface that have previously been found to respond to different non-visual tasks in blind adults and show different functional connectivity across blind and sighted adults (Kanjlia et al., 2016, 2021; Lane et al., 2015). In blind adults, these three regions respond to different higher-cognitive tasks, including language, numerical reasoning and executive control (Kanjlia et al., 2016, 2021; Lane et al., 2015). In sighted people, these regions roughly correspond to the anatomical location of areas such as motion area V5/MT+, the lateral occipital complex (LO), V3a and V4v (Tootell et al., 1997; Van Essen et al., 2001). Although their precise visual function in sighted people is not known. Together, these three regions tile the majority of lateral occipital cortex, providing a good sample of the connectivity profile of higher-order visual areas.
We also examined connectivity of primary visual cortex (V1), which likewise shows altered task-based responses and functional connectivity in congenitally blind adults (Amedi et al., 2003; Bedny et al., 2011; Burton et al., 2014; Butt et al., 2013; Lane et al., 2015; N. Raz et al., 2005; Sadato et al., 1996; Striem-Amit et al., 2015; Yu et al., 2008). Since many previous studies have found that blindness alters the balance of connectivity between visual cortex and higher-order prefrontal as opposed to sensory-motor regions, this was our primary outcome measure. We also examined changes in connectivity lateralization – i.e., the balance of between vs. within hemisphere connectivity.
To preview the results, we find that the functional connectivity of secondary visual areas in infants resembles that of blind more than sighted adults, whereas V1 of infants falls between the two adult populations. This suggests that vision plays an instructive role in setting up the balance of connectivity between visual cortex and non-visual networks. In contrast, lateralization patterns appear to reflect blindness-related reorganization.
Results
Connectivity profile of secondary visual cortices in sighted infants is more similar to that of blind than sighted adults
Secondary visual areas of sighted adults showed stronger functional connectivity with non-visual sensory areas (primary somatosensory and motor cortex, S1/M1, and primary auditory cortex, A1) than with prefrontal cortices (PFC). By contrast, in blind adults, visual cortices showed higher functional connectivity with PFC than with non-visual sensory areas (S1/M1 and A1) (group (sighted adults, blind adults) by ROI (PFC, non-visual sensory) interaction effect: F(1, 78) = 148.819, p < 0.001; post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected paired t-test, sighted adults: non-visual sensory > PFC: t (49) = 9.722, p < 0.001; blind adults: non-visual sensory < PFC: t(29) =8.852, p < 0.001; Fig. 1).
Like in blind adults, in sighted infants, secondary visual cortices showed higher connectivity to PFC than non-visual sensory areas (S1/M1 and A1) (non-visual sensory < PFC paired t-test, t (474) = 20.144, p < 0.001) (Fig. 1). The connectivity matrix of sighted infants was also more correlated with that of blind than sighted adults, but strongly correlated with both adult groups (secondary visual, PFC and non-visual sensory areas: sighted infants correlated to blind adults: r = 0.721, p < 0.001; to sighted adults: r = 0.524, p < 0.001; difference between correlations of infants to blind vs. to sighted adults: z = 3.77, p < 0.001; see Supplementary Figure S1 for the connectivity matrices).
These results suggests that vision is required to set up the sighted adult functional connectivity pattern, i.e., vision enhances occipital cortex connectivity to non-visual sensory-motor networks and dampens connectivity to higher-cognitive prefrontal networks.
We checked the robustness of these results in a number of ways. First, we compared the effects across the three different secondary visual regions and found that the same pattern held across all three regions (Supplementary results and Supplementary Figure S2). Next, to check the robustness of the findings in infants we randomly split the infant dataset into two halves and did split-half cross-validation. Across all comparisons the results of the two halves were highly similar, suggesting the effects are robust (see Supplementary Figure S3 to Supplementary Figure S6). We performed this validation procedure for all analyses reported below with similar results.
The connectivity pattern of V1 influenced both by early visual experience and blindness
As for secondary visual corticies, we examined the functional connectivity of the primary visual cortex (V1) with non-visual sensory areas (S1/M1 and A1) and PFC. V1 showed the same dissociation between sighted and blind adults as secondary visual areas: in sighted adults, V1 has stronger functional connectivity with non-visual sensory areas than with PFC. By contrast, in blind adults, V1 shows stronger connectivity with PFC than with non-visual sensory areas (group (sighted adults, blind adults) by ROI (PFC, non-visual sensory) interaction: F(1, 78) = 125.775, p < 0.001; post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected paired t-test, sighted adults non-visual sensory > PFC: t (49) = 9.404, p < 0.001; blind adults non-visual sensory < PFC: t (29) =7.128, p < 0.001; Fig. 2).
The pattern for sighted infants in V1 fell between that of sighted and blind adults. The connectivity matrix of sighted infants (V1, PFC, and non-visual sensory) was equally correlated with blind and sighted adults (infants correlated to blind adults: r = 0.654, p < 0.001; to sighted adults: r = 0.594, p < 0.001; correlation of infants with blind vs. with sighted adults: z = 0.832, p = 0.406; see Supplementary Figure S1 for the connectivity matrices). The difference in connectivity strength between V1 to PFC and V1 to non-visual sensory regions was weaker in sighted infants than in sighted or blind adults (group (sighted adults, infants) by ROI (PFC, non-visual sensory) interaction effect: F(1, 523) = 92.21, p < 0.001; group (blind adults, infants) by ROI (PFC, non-visual sensory) interaction effect: F(1, 503) = 57.444, p < 0.001). V1 of sighted infants showed marginally stronger connectivity to non-visual sensory regions (S1/M1 and A1) than PFC (non-visual sensory regions > PFC, paired t-test, t (474) = 1.95, p = 0.052; Fig.2).
The dHCP cohort included both full-term neonates and preterm infants, scanned at their equivalent gestational age. Visual exposure therefore varied somewhat in duration across infants (from 0 to 19.71 weeks), with slightly longer exposure in preterm babies. This variation did not affect connectivity patterns either in V1 or secondary visual cortices (V1: r = 0.06, p = 0.192; secondary visual: r = 0.004, p = 0.923; see Supplementary Figure S8). A few weeks of vision after birth is therefore insufficient to influence connectivity.
Evidence for blindness-related reorganization in laterality of occipito-frontal connectivity
Relative to sighted adults, blind adults showed a stronger dominance of within hemisphere connectivity between secondary and primary visual cortices and PFC. That is, in people born blind, left visual networks are more strongly connected to left PFC networks, whereas right visual networks are more strongly connected to right PFC. By contrast, there is no hemispheric bias in the sighted group (group (blind adults, sighted adults) by lateralization (within hemisphere, between hemisphere) interaction, secondary visual cortices: F(1, 78) = 131.51, p < 0.001; post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected paired: t-test: sighted adults within hemisphere > across hemisphere: t (49) = 5.778, p < 0.001; blind adults within hemisphere > across hemisphere: t(29) = 10.735, p < 0.001; V1: F(1, 78) = 87.211, p < 0.001; post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected paired: t-test: sighted adults within hemisphere > between hemisphere: t (49) = 3.251, p = 0.101; blind adults within hemisphere > between hemisphere: t (29) = 7.019, p < 0.001).
These connectivity results are consistent with previous studies of task-based cross-modal responses in blindness. In blind adults cross-modal responses in occipital cortex and co-lateralize with fronto-parietal networks with related functions (Kanjlia et al., 2021; Lane et al., 2017). For example, language-responsive occipital areas collateralize with language responsive prefrontal areas across individuals (Lane et al., 2017).
The connectivity pattern of sighted infants resembled sighted more than blind adults (Fig. 3), suggesting blindness-driven reorganization. There was a significant difference in laterality between blind adults and sighted infants (group (blind adults, infants) by lateralization (within hemisphere, between hemisphere) interaction effect: F(1, 503) = 303.04, p < 0.001). By contrast, there was no difference between sighted adults and sighted infants (group (sighted adults, infants) by lateralization (within hemisphere, across hemisphere) interaction effect: F(1, 523) = 2.244, p = 0.135; see supplementary results for a detailed group comparison of within and across hemisphere differences). Similar group by laterality interaction pattern are also observed in V1 (group (blind adults, infants) by lateralization (within hemisphere, between hemisphere) interaction effect: F(1, 503) = 123.608, p < 0.001; group (sighted adults, infants) by lateralization (within hemisphere, across hemisphere) interaction effect: F(1, 523) = 2.827, p = 0.093). The incorporation of visual cortices into lateralized functional networks (e.g., language, response selection) in blindness, which is observed in task-based studies, may drive stronger within-hemisphere connectivity in this population (Kanjlia et al., 2021; Lane et al., 2017; Tian et al., 2022).
Specialization across different fronto-occipital networks: present in adults, absent at birth
It is not the case that every occipital area shows equal connectivity to every prefrontal area. On the contrary, in blind adults resting state connectivity patterns are specialized and aligned with the functional specialization observed in task-based data (Bedny et al., 2011; Kanjlia et al., 2016, 2021). For example, language-responsive subregions of occipital cortex show strongest functional connectivity with language-responsive sub-regions of PFC, whereas math-responsive occipital areas show stronger connectivity with math-responsive PFC (Fig. 4) (Bedny et al., 2011; Kanjlia et al., 2016, 2021; Lane et al., 2015). Is this fronto-occipital connectivity specialization present in infancy, potentially driving task-based cross-modal specialization?
We compared connectivity preferences across three prefrontal and three occipital regions previous shown to activate preferentially in language (sentences > math), math (math > sentences) and response-conflict (no-go > go with tones) tasks respectively (Kanjlia et al., 2016, 2021; Lane et al., 2015). For ease of viewing, Fig. 4 shows results from two of the three regions, math and language. However, all statistical analyses included all three areas (See Supplementary Figure S9 for all three regions).
Sighted infants showed a less differentiated fronto-occipital connectivity pattern relative to sighted and blind adults (Group (sighted adults, blind adults, sighted infants) by occipital regions (math, language, response-conflict) by PFC regions (math, language, response-conflict) interaction F(8, 2208) = 16.323, p < 0.001). Unlike in adults, in infants, all the occipital regions showed stronger correlations with math- and response-conflict related prefrontal areas than language-responsive prefrontal areas (Fig. 4 and Supplementary Figure S9). The occipital region that is sensitive to response-conflict in blind adults showed equivalent correlations with math and response-conflict PFC regions in infants. The region of occipital cortex that responds to language and shows the strongest connectivity with language responsive PFC in blind adults, showed stronger connectivity with math and response-conflict PFC areas in infants.
Although fronto-occipital connectivity was not adult-like in infants, biases in infants were somewhat consistent with future differentiation: the preferential correlation with math responsive PFC was strongest in those occipital areas that go on to develop math responses in blind adults (occipital regions (math, language, response-conflict) by PFC regions (math, language, response-conflict) interaction in infants F(4, 1896) = 85.145, p < 0.001, post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected paired t-test see Supplementary Table 1). Although the occipital region that is language-responsive in blind adults showed stronger connectivity to math and response-conflict areas of PFC in infants, this biased against language areas was smaller for this occipital areas than the other two.
Note that findings regarding regional specialization need to be interpreted with caution for two reasons. First, although, prior evidence suggests that at least some of the prefrontal specialization present in adults is already present even in young babies, we do not know whether the language/number/executive function distinctions exist in prefrontal cortices of infants (Raz & Saxe, 2020). Second, these more fine-grained comparisons across occipital/frontal regions are more vulnerable to potential anatomical alignment issues between adult and infant brains. In other words, lack of specialization in infants could reflect the different location of the areas in this population.
Discussion
The sighted adult functional connectivity pattern, although the most common in the population, is not the ‘default’ starting state in infants but rather requires visual experience to establish. This was particularly evident in the case of secondary visual cortices, where, resting state connectivity patterns with non-visual networks in sighted infants resemble those of blind adults more so than those of sighted adults. Both in infants and blind adults, secondary occipital areas showed stronger functional connectivity with higher-order prefrontal cortices than with other sensory-motor networks (S1/M1, A1). Consistent with this observation, one previous study with a small sample of infants found strong connectivity between lateral occipital and prefrontal areas, although there was no comparison to blind adults in that study (Barttfeld et al., 2018). In V1, infants fell somewhere in between sighted and blind adults, suggesting an effect both of vision and of blindness on functional connectivity. We hypothesize that vision, as well as temporally coordinated multi-modal experiences contribute to establishing the sighted connectivity profile.
Since visual behavior is likely to be influenced by the communication of visual cortices with non-visual networks, a key question concerns the behavioral relevance of these connectivity signatures for vision and multimodal integration. An increasing number of people who grew up blind will have the possibility of sight restoration in adulthood e.g., through cataract removal, corneal transplant or gene therapy. Recent evidence suggests that sight recovery individuals show some multimodal integration deficits (Ashtari, 2020; Badde et al., 2020; Guerreiro et al., 2015; Putzar et al., 2007). There is also evidence that occipital oscillations, which affect cross-network communication, are different in this population (Pant et al., 2023). It will therefore be important to determine the contribution of connectivity to visual and multimodal sensory behavior and how connectivity might be shaped later in life.
For people who remain blind throughout life, the infant connectivity profile could play a role in enabling recruitment of visual cortices by non-visual functions, such as language and non-verbal executive processes. Habitual activation of occipital networks during higher cognitive tasks in early development could then itself influence both connectivity and selectivity, resulting in different adult profiles.
The clearest evidence for blindness-related reorganization in the current study was observed in the case of laterality. Connectivity lateralization in sighted infants resembles that of sighted adults, in both V1 and secondary visual cortices. Said differently, relative to both sighted infants and sighted adults, blind adults show more lateralized connectivity patterns between occipital and prefrontal cortices. Previous studies suggest that in people born blind occipital and non-occipital language responses are co-lateralized (Lane et al., 2017). We speculate that habitual activation of visual cortices by higher-cognitive tasks, such as language, which are themselves highly lateralized, contributes to this biased connectivity pattern of occipital cortex in blindness.
Materials and methods
Participants
Fifty sighted adults and thirty congenitally blind adults contributed the resting state data (sighted: n = 50; 30 females; mean age = 35.33 years, standard deviation (SD) = 14.65; mean years of education = 17.08, SD = 3.1; blind: n = 30; 19 females; mean age = 44.23 years, SD = 16.41; mean years of education = 17.08, SD = 2.11; blind vs. sighted age, t (78) = 2.512, p < 0.05; blind vs. sighted years of education, t (78) = 0.05, p = 0.996). Since blind participants were on average older, we also performed analyses in an age-matched subgroups of sighted controls (n = 29) and found similar results to the full sample (see Supplementary Figure S11 to Figure S14). Blind and sighted participants had no known cognitive or neurological disabilities (screened through self-report). All adult anatomical images were read by a board-certified radiologist and no gross neurological abnormalities were found. All the blind participants had at most minimal light perception from birth. Blindness was caused by pathology anterior to the optic chiasm (i.e., not due to brain damage). All participants gave written informed consent under a protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board of Johns Hopkins University.
Neonate data were from the third release of the Developing Human Connectome Project (dHCP) (n = 783) (https://www.developingconnectome.org). Ethical approval was obtained from the UK Health Research Authority (Research Ethics Committee reference number: 14/LO/1169). After quality control procedures (described below), 475 subjects were included in data analysis, with one scan per subject. The average age from birth at scan = 2.79 weeks (SD = 3.77); average gestational age at scan = 41.23 weeks (SD = 1.77). We only included infants who were full-term or scanned at term-equivalent age if preterm, while not being flagged by the dHCP project team as not passing quality control for functional MRI (fMRI) images (n = 634). Infants with more than 160 motion outliers were exclude (n = 116 dropped). Motion-outlier volumes were defined as DVARS (the root mean square intensity difference between successive volumes) higher than 1.5 interquartile range above the 75th centile, after motion and distortion correction. Infants with signal drop-out in regions of interest (ROI) were also excluded (n = 43 dropped). To identify signal dropout, we first averaged blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal intensity for all time point, for each subject, in each of 100 parcel defined by Schaefer’s atlas (Schaefer et al., 2018). For each ROI (n = 18 ROIs) in the current study, signal dropout was then identified as BOLD intensity lower than −3 standard deviations, where the mean and standard deviations were identified across all 100 cortical parcels. Participants were excluded if any of the ROIs showed a signal dropout.
Image acquisition
Blind and sighted adult
MRI anatomical and functional images were collected on a 3T Phillips scanner at the F. M. Kirby Research Center. T1-weighted anatomical images were collected using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) in 150 axial slices with 1 mm isotropic voxels. Resting state fMRI data were collected in 36 sequential ascending axial slices for 8 minutes. TR = 2 s, TE = 0.03 s, flip angle = 70°, voxel size = 2.4 × 2.4 × 2.5 mm, inter-slice gap = 0.5 mm, field of view (FOV) = 192 × 172.8 × 107.5. Participants completed 1 to 4 scans of 240 volume each (average scan time = 710.4 second per person). During the resting state scan, participants were instructed to relax but remain awake. Sighted participants wore light-excluding blindfolds to equalize the light conditions across the groups during the scans.
Infants (dHCP)
Anatomical and functional images were collected on a 3T Phillips scanner at the Evelina Newborn Imaging Centre, St Thomas’ Hospital, London, UK. A dedicated neonatal imaging 219 system including a neonatal 32-channel phased-array head coil was used. T2w multi-slice fast spin-echo images were acquired with in-plane resolution 0.8x0.8 mm2 and 1.6 mm slices overlapped by 0.8 mm (TR = 12000 ms, TE = 156 ms, SENSE factor 2.11 axial and 2.6 sagittal). In infants, T2w images were used as the anatomical image because the brain anatomy is more clearly in T2w than in T1w images. Fifteen minutes of resting state fMRI data were collected using a used multiband 9x accelerated echo-planar imaging (TR = 392 ms, TE = 38 ms, 2300 volumes, with an acquired resolution of 2.15 mm isotropic). Single-band reference scans were acquired with bandwidth-matched readout, along with additional spin-echo acquisitions with both AP/PA fold-over encoding directions.
Data analysis
Resting state data were preprocessed using FSL version 5.0.9 (Smith et al., 2004), DPABI version 6.1 (Yan et al., 2016), FreeSurfer (Dale et al., 1999), and in-house code (https://github.com/NPDL/Resting-state_dHCP). The functional data for all groups were linearly detrended and low-pass filtered (0.08 Hz).
For adults, functional images were registered to the T1-weighted structural images, motion corrected using MCFLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002), and temporally high-pass filtering (150 s). No subject had excessive head movement (> 2mm) or rotation (> 2°) at any timepoint. Resting state data are known to include artifacts related to physiological fluctuations such as cardiac pulsations and respiratory-induced modulation of the main magnetic field. A component-based method, CompCor (Behzadi et al., 2007), was therefore used to control for these artifacts. Particularly, following the procedure described in Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., nuisance signals were extracted from 2-voxel eroded masks of spinal fluid (CSF) and white matter (WM), and the first 5 principal components analysis (PCA) components derived from these signals was regressed out from the processed BOLD time series (Whitfield-Gabrieli & Nieto-Castanon, 2012). In addition, a scrubbing procedure was applied to further reduce the effect of motion on functional connectivity measures (Power et al., 2012, 2014). Frames with root mean square intensity difference exceeding 1.5 interquartile range above the 75th centile, after motion and distortion correction, were censored as outliers.
The infants resting state functional data were pre-processed by the dHCP group using the project’s in-house pipeline (Fitzgibbon et al., 2020), This pipeline uses a spatial independent component analysis (ICA) denoising step to minimize artifact due to multi-band artefact, residual head-movement, arteries, sagittal sinus, CSF pulsation. For infants, ICA denoising is preferable to using CSF/WM regressors. Because it is challenging to accurately define anatomical boundaries of CSF/WM due to the low imaging resolution comparing with the brain size and the severe partial-volume effect in the neonate (Fitzgibbon et al., 2020). Like in the adults, frames with root mean square intensity difference exceeding 1.5 interquartile range above the 75th centile, after motion and distortion correction, were considered as motion outliers. Out from the 2300 frames, a subset of continuous 1600 with minimum number of motion outliers was kept for each subject. Motion outliers were censored from the subset of continuous 1600, and a subject was excluded from further analyses when the number of outlier exceeded 160 (10% of the continues subset) (Hu et al., 2022).
For both groups of adult and infants, we performed a temporal low-pass filter (0.08 Hz low-pass cutoff) and a linear detrending. ROI-to-ROI connectivity was calculated using Pearson’s correlation between ROI-averaged BOLD timeseries (ROI definition see below). The All t-tests and F-tests are two-sided. The comparison of correlation coefficients was done using cocor software package and Pearson and Filon’s z (Diedenhofen & Musch, 2015; Pearson & Filon, 1898).
ROI definition
ROIs in the frontal and occipital cortices were defined from separate task-based fMRI experiments with blind and sighted adults (Kanjlia et al., 2016, 2021; Lane et al., 2015). Three separate experiments were conducted with the same group of blind and sighted subjects (sighted n=18; blind n=23). The language ROIs in the occipital and frontal cortices were identified by sentence > nonwords contrast in an auditory language comprehension task (Lane et al., 2015). The math ROIs were identified by math > sentence contrast in an auditory task where participants judged equivalence of pairs of math equations and pairs of sentences (Kanjlia et al., 2016). The executive function ROIs were identified by no-go > frequent go contrast in an auditory go/no-go task with non-verbal sounds (Kanjlia et al., 2021). The occipital ROIs were defined based on group comparisons blind > sighted in a whole-cortex analysis. For example, the occipital language ROI were defined as the cluster that responded more to auditory sentence than auditory nonwords conditions in blind, relative to sighted, in a whole-cortex analysis. All three occipital ROIs were defined in the right hemisphere. Left-hemisphere occipital ROIs were created by flipping the right-hemisphere ROIs to the left hemisphere. The frontal ROIs were defined based on a whole-cortex analysis which combined all blind and sighted adult data. For example, the frontal language ROI was defined as responded more auditory sentence than auditory nonwords conditions across all blind and sighted subjects, constrained to the prefrontal cortex. For frontal ROIs, the language ROI was defined in the left, and the math and executive function ROI were defined in the right hemisphere, then flip to the other hemisphere.
The V1 was defined from a previously published anatomical surface-based atlas (PALS-B12) (Van Essen, 2005). The primary somatosensory and motor cortex (S1/M1) ROI was selected as the area that responds more to the go than no-go trials in the auditory go/no-go task across both blind and sighted groups, constrained to the hand area in S1/M1 search-space from neurosynth.org (term “hand movements”) (Kanjlia et al., 2021). The primary auditory cortex (A1) ROI was defined as the transverse temporal portion of a gyral-based atlas (Desikan et al., 2006; Morosan et al., 2001).
All the ROIs were defined in standard space. For infants, the ROIs were subsequently transformed into each subject’s native space using a two-step approach. First, the ROIs were converted from the adult’s MNI space into the 40-week dHCP template (Bozek et al., 2018). ANTS, previously shown to be effective in pediatric studies (Avants et al., 2014; Cabral et al., 2022; Jain et al., 2012; Lawson et al., 2013), was utilized to estimate the deformation field between these two spaces. In this step, the infant’s scalp and cerebellum were masked, as these structures in the infant brain greatly differ from those in the adult and can introduce bias into the registration process, as outlined in a study by Cabral et al (Cabral et al., 2022). Secondly, the ROIs were further transformed from the 40-week space into individual’s native spaces, employing the deformation field provided by the dHCP group. Nearest neighbor interpolation was applied in both steps (see examples of ROI in individual infant brains in the Supplementary Figure S15). For adults, the ROIs were also transformed into each subject’s native space, employing the deformation field estimated by FreeSurfer. For both adults and infants, any overlapping voxels between ROIs were removed and not counted toward any ROIs.
Data availability
Neonate data were from the second and third release of the Developing Human Connectome Project (https://www.developingconnectome.org). The de-identified blind and sighted adults’ data will be posted on Vivli.org upon publication of the current manuscript.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank all the blind and sighted participants, the blind community and the National Federation of the Blind. Without their support, this study would not be possible. We would also like to thank the F. M. Kirby Research Center for Functional Brain Imaging at the Kennedy Krieger Institute for their assistance in data collection. Xiang Xiao was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the National Institute of Health, United States.
Funding
This work was supported by grants from the National Eye Institute at the National Institutes of Health (R01EY027352-01 and R01EY033340). RC was supported by the ERC Advanced Grant “Foundations of Cognition” (FOUNDCOG) 787981.
Competing interests
The authors report no competing interests.
References
- Distinctive Interaction Between Cognitive Networks and the Visual Cortex in Early Blind IndividualsCerebral Cortex 33:4725–4742https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz006
- Early “visual” cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blindNature Neuroscience 6:758–766https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1072
- Compensatory Cross-Modal Plasticity Persists After Sight RestorationFrontiers in Neuroscience 14
- The Insight ToolKit image registration frameworkFrontiers in Neuroinformatics 8
- Sensory experience during early sensitive periods shapes cross-modal temporal biasesELife 9https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.61238
- A lateral-to-mesial organization of human ventral visual cortex at birthBrain Structure and Function 223:3107–3119https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-018-1676-3
- Language processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind adultsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108:4429–4434https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014818108
- A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRINeuroImage 37:90–101https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042
- Construction of a neonatal cortical surface atlas using Multimodal Surface Matching in the Developing Human Connectome ProjectNeuroImage 179:11–29https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.018
- Recognition memory for Braille or spoken words: An fMRI study in early blindBrain Research 1438:22–34https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.12.032
- Resting state functional connectivity in early blind humansFrontiers in Systems Neuroscience https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00051
- The fine-scale functional correlation of striate cortex in sighted and blind peopleJournal of Neuroscience 33:16209–16219https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0363-13.2013
- Anatomical correlates of category-selective visual regions have distinctive signatures of connectivity in neonatesDevelopmental Cognitive Neuroscience 58https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101179
- Functional specialization for auditory-spatial processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind humansProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108:4435–4440https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1013928108
- Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstructionNeuroImage 9:179–194https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0395
- Occipital Cortex of Blind Individuals Is Functionally Coupled with Executive Control Areas of Frontal CortexJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27:1633–1647https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00807
- An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interestNeuroImage 31:968–980https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021
- cocor: A Comprehensive Solution for the Statistical Comparison of CorrelationsPLOS ONE 10https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121945
- Emergence of resting state networks in the preterm human brainProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107:20015–20020https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1007921107
- The developing Human Connectome Project (dHCP) automated resting-state functional processing framework for newborn infantsNeuroImage 223https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117303
- Spontaneous brain activity in the newborn brain during natural sleep-an fMRI study in infants born at full termPediatric Research 66:301–305https://doi.org/10.1203/PDR.0b013e3181b1bd84
- Evidence on the emergence of the brain’s default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjectsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106:6790–6795https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0811221106
- The effect of early visual deprivation on the neural bases of multisensory processingBrain 138:1499–1504https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awv076
- Typical and disrupted brain circuitry for conscious awareness in full-term and preterm infantsBrain Communications 4https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcac071
- Longitudinal Reproducibility and Accuracy of Pseudo-Continuous Arterial Spin–labeled Perfusion MR Imaging in Typically Developing ChildrenRadiology 263:527–536https://doi.org/10.1148/radiol.12111509
- Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain imagesNeuroImage 17:825–841https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(02)91132-8
- Absence of visual experience modifies the neural basis of numerical thinkingProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113:11172–11177https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1524982113
- ‘Visual’ cortices of congenitally blind adults are sensitive to response selection demands in a go/no-go taskNeuroImage 236https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118023
- “Visual” cortex of congenitally blind adults responds to syntactic movementJournal of Neuroscience 35:12859–12868https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1256-15.2015
- Reduced Left Lateralization of Language in Congenitally Blind IndividualsJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 29:65–78https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01045
- Associations between children’s socioeconomic status and prefrontal cortical thicknessDevelopmental Science 16:641–652https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12096
- Functional connectivity of the sensorimotor area in naturally sleeping infantsBrain Research 1223:42–49https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.05.054
- Whole brain functional connectivity in the early blindBrain 130:2085–2096https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm121
- V1 Projection Zone Signals in Human Macular Degeneration Depend on Task Despite Absence of Visual StimulusCurrent Biology 31:406–412https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.10.034
- Human primary auditory cortex: Cytoarchitectonic subdivisions and mapping into a spatial reference systemNeuroImage 13:684–701https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0715
- Stimulus-evoked and resting-state alpha oscillations show a linked dependence on patterned visual experience for developmentNeuroImage: Clinical 38https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2023.103375
- Mathematical contributions to the theory of evolutionIV. On the probable errors of frequency constants and on the influence of random selection on variation and correlation. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 62:173–176https://doi.org/10.1098/rspl.1897.0091
- Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motionNeuroImage 59:2142–2154https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018
- Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRINeuroImage 84:320–341https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048
- Early visual deprivation impairs multisensory interactions in humansNature Neuroscience 10:1243–1245https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1978
- Learning in Infancy Is ActiveEndogenously Motivated, and Depends on the Prefrontal Cortices. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology 2:247–268https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084841
- V1 activation in congenitally blind humans is associated with episodic retrievalCerebral Cortex 15:1459–1468https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi026
- Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjectsNature 380:526–528https://doi.org/10.1038/380526a0
- Local-Global Parcellation of the Human Cerebral Cortex from Intrinsic Functional Connectivity MRICerebral Cortex 28:3095–3114https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx179
- Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSLNeuroImage 23:208–219https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051
- Functional connectivity of visual cortex in the blind follows retinotopic organization principlesBrain 138:1679–1695https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awv083
- Sensory modality and spoken language shape reading network in blind readers of BrailleCerebral Cortex
- Functional Analysis of V3A and Related Areas in Human Visual CortexJournal of Neuroscience 17:7060–7078https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-18-07060.1997
- A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortexNeuroImage 28:635–662https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.058
- Mapping visual cortex in monkeys and humans using surface-based atlasesVision Research 41:1359–1378https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00045-1
- Language networks in anophthalmia: Maintained hierarchy of processing in “visual” cortexBrain 135:1566–1577https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws067
- Conn: A Functional Connectivity Toolbox for Correlated and Anticorrelated Brain NetworksBrain Connectivity 2:125–141https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2012.0073
- DPABI: Data Processing & Analysis for (Resting-State) Brain ImagingNeuroinformatics 14:339–351https://doi.org/10.1007/s12021-016-9299-4
- Altered functional connectivity of primary visual cortex in early blindnessHuman Brain Mapping 29:533–543https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20420
- Resting-state functional MRI studies on infant brains: A decade of gap-filling effortsNeuroImage 185https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.004
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Copyright
© 2024, Tian et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 349
- downloads
- 27
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.