Locomotion: How vision shapes the paths we choose
When walking, our brain needs to make decisions, such as where to step and which path to follow. Visual inputs are central to this process, as they allow us to continuously estimate our motion and orientation, as well as regulate our direction and speed (Warren et al., 2001).
This continuous regulation can be explained using mathematical models that rely on vector calculus (Matthis et al., 2022). However, walking also involves discrete choices – such as selecting which available foothold to use, or turning left or right – that are guided by visual features of the path ahead, such as the rockiness and steepness of the terrain (Matthis et al., 2018). While it may seem obvious that people avoid rugged paths when easier alternatives exist, there is no mathematical model to explain this behavior. Now, in eLife, Mary Hayhoe and colleagues – including Karl Muller as first author – report the results of a study that sheds new light on how these decisions are made (Muller et al., 2024).
The researchers analyzed data from previous studies in which they had tracked the eye and body movements of nine individuals as they walked across natural terrains (Matthis et al., 2022; Bonnen et al., 2021). Participants also wore eye-tracking cameras that captured visual information about where they were looking within the surrounding physical landscape. Muller et al. used this data to create a three-dimensional computational reconstruction of the terrains, which they aligned to the body and eye movements of the participants. This allowed them to develop egocentric visual depth images (which estimated how the retinas of individuals detect the depth of the terrain), and to determine where participants placed their feet.
The researchers – who are based at the University of Texas at Austin, Indiana University and Northeastern University – then used these visual depth images and foothold locations to train a neural network where to step in the reconstructed terrain. The trained model successfully predicted where the human participants placed their feet better than chance, suggesting that decisions about where to step are influenced by visual depth cues. This agrees with prior work showing reduced depth cues cause walkers to shift their gaze to closer footholds, perhaps so they can devote more attention to resolving depth ambiguities (Bonnen et al., 2021).
Next, Muller et al. used their model to investigate whether walkers chose flatter, more winding paths over direct, steeper alternatives that require more energy to cross due to the uneven terrain (Darici and Kuo, 2023; Kowalsky et al., 2021). The team compared two metrics: tortuosity, which is the ratio between the length of a path and the direct distance between its start and end; and a dimensionless measure of ruggedness, equal to the average angle of the up and down steps within a path. These metrics were recorded for the actual path the walkers took, and also for simulated, alternative routes they might have taken through the same terrain.
Muller et al. found that the tortuosity of the paths chosen by the human walkers correlated with the ruggedness of the most direct simulated path through the terrain: the steeper and more rugged the direct path, the more likely a walker is to detour and take a longer, flatter route. This suggests a trade-off whereby walkers assess paths with high tortuosity to be less energetically costly than ones which are straighter but more rugged. Muller et al. also discovered that this trade-off was stronger for shorter individuals, possibly due to greater difficulty with steep steps, though this effect warrants further study (as discussed in the public reviews of the article).
Studying locomotion in natural settings is challenging, as terrains and visual features are harder to manipulate or measure compared to controlled laboratory environments. The methods created by Muller et al. open up new avenues for understanding decision-making during locomotion. However, there is much about the trade-off proposed in the study that remains unknown.
For example, controlled experiments are needed to confirm whether the ruggedness of the simulated direct paths match those experienced in real life. In addition, the human eye likely perceives other aspects of the terrain beyond just depth, and it remains to be seen whether other visual cues help evaluate different structural properties of the landscape, such as friction and the hardness of footholds. Related issues in natural locomotion, such as energy expenditure, also merit further investigation. For instance, the energetic costs of tortuous versus rugged paths still need quantification, as do temporal costs, which are also suspected to govern ecological behavior (Carlisle and Kuo, 2023).
The approach taken by Muller et al. could also be used to study people in human-designed environments, such as pedestrians crossing busy roads (do people jaywalk rather than use pedestrian crossings to save energy, time, or other costs?). Whether navigating natural landscapes or cityscapes, humans constantly create their own paths according to a variety of physical and perceptual criteria.
References
-
Gaze and the control of foot placement when walking in natural terrainCurrent Biology 28:1224–1233.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.03.008
-
Retinal optic flow during natural locomotionPLOS Computational Biology 18:e1009575.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009575
-
Optic flow is used to control human walkingNature Neuroscience 4:213–216.https://doi.org/10.1038/84054
Article and author information
Author details
Publication history
Copyright
© 2024, Kuo
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 217
- views
-
- 10
- downloads
-
- 0
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Neural diversity can expand the encoding capacity of a circuitry. A striking example of diverse structure and function is presented by the afferent synapses between inner hair cells (IHCs) and spiral ganglion neurons (SGNs) in the cochlea. Presynaptic active zones at the pillar IHC side activate at lower IHC potentials than those of the modiolar side that have more presynaptic Ca2+ channels. The postsynaptic SGNs differ in their spontaneous firing rates, sound thresholds, and operating ranges. While a causal relationship between synaptic heterogeneity and neural response diversity seems likely, experimental evidence linking synaptic and SGN physiology has remained difficult to obtain. Here, we aimed at bridging this gap by ex vivo paired recordings of murine IHCs and postsynaptic SGN boutons with stimuli and conditions aimed to mimic those of in vivo SGN characterization. Synapses with high spontaneous rate of release (SR) were found predominantly on the pillar side of the IHC. These high SR synapses had larger and more temporally compact spontaneous EPSCs, lower voltage thresholds, tighter coupling of Ca2+ channels and vesicular release sites, shorter response latencies, and higher initial release rates. This study indicates that synaptic heterogeneity in IHCs directly contributes to the diversity of spontaneous and sound-evoked firing of SGNs.
-
- Neuroscience
Life histories of oviparous species dictate high metabolic investment in the process of gonadal development leading to ovulation. In vertebrates, these two distinct processes are controlled by the gonadotropins follicle-stimulating hormone (FSH) and luteinizing hormone (LH), respectively. While it was suggested that a common secretagogue, gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH), oversees both functions, the generation of loss-of-function fish challenged this view. Here, we reveal that the satiety hormone cholecystokinin (CCK) is the primary regulator of this axis in zebrafish. We found that FSH cells express a CCK receptor, and our findings demonstrate that mutating this receptor results in a severe hindrance to ovarian development. Additionally, it causes a complete shutdown of both gonadotropins secretion. Using in-vivo and ex-vivo calcium imaging of gonadotrophs, we show that GnRH predominantly activates LH cells, whereas FSH cells respond to CCK stimulation, designating CCK as the bona fide FSH secretagogue. These findings indicate that the control of gametogenesis in fish was placed under different neural circuits, that are gated by CCK.