Echolocation: Smart bats click twice

The acoustic representation of the outside world in the midbrain of a bat becomes more precise as it uses double clicks to locate closer objects.
  1. Manfred Kössl  Is a corresponding author
  2. Julio Hechavarría  Is a corresponding author
  1. Goethe University, Germany

To navigate their environment, humans and other animals need to be aware of their surroundings and their position relative to other objects. This ability to perceive space is crucial for survival and usually relies on all our senses conveying sensory information to the brain, which computes a model of the outside world that enables the animals to act accordingly. While humans gather acoustic and visual information ‘passively’, some animals have developed sensory systems that actively explore their surroudings.

For example, bats use a process called echolocation to orient themselves in space and to search for food. This involves emitting high-frequency sound waves from their nose or mouth, and then detecting the echoes of these waves after they have been reflected by a nearby object (Figure 1). The time taken for the echo to reach the bat helps it to estimate the distance and direction of the object and to create a direct perception of the depth of the space that surrounds them.

Different approaches to echolocation.

Bats use sound waves to see in the dark. Most of the time, big brown bats emit a single 'click' of sound (as shown on the left of the rectangle labelled vocalizations), and a single spike is generated in the brain when the echo of this click is detected (as can be seen on the left of the rectangle labelled neural signal). However, when a bat gets close to an object it emits a double click, which leads to the generation of a more complex signal in the brain (three spikes in this case). Kothari et al. found that during double clicking a group of neurons in the superior colliculus, which is in the midbrain, responded more strongly to close objects. Image credit: Melville Wohlgemuth.

Most previous research in this area has focused on how the brain processes either the direction or the distance when the bats are restrained in some way. Two regions in the brain are thought to be involved in encoding spatial depth: the auditory cortex, and a region in the midbrain called the inferior colliculus (Suga and O'Neill, 1979; Wenstrup and Portfors, 2011). However, when the bats are free to move, the distances and directions of objects change constantly, and relatively little is known about how the brain deals with these changes.

Now, in eLife, Ninad Kothari, Melville Wohlgemuth and Cynthia Moss at Johns Hopkins University report that another region in the midbrain, the superior colliculus, plays an important role in estimating distance (Kothari et al., 2018). The superior colliculus is known to merge visual and auditory information and to direct behavioral responses towards stimuli in the environment. In bats, it is thought to be involved in controlling sensory-guided behaviors that help the animals to orientate themselves by using acoustic signals or adjusting their body or ears towards objects detected by echolocation (Covey et al., 1987; Valentine et al., 2002).

Kothari et al. inserted wireless probes into the midbrain region of bats and recorded the electrical impulses in their brains as they flew in three-dimensional space; the echolocation signals were also recorded. The researchers discovered a group of neurons in the superior colliculus that compute a three-dimensional representation of the space around the bat by processing information about the distance in between objects.

Bats can control their ‘acoustic view’ of the world by changing the temporal structure of their echolocation streams. At closer distances, they often use pairs of echolocation signals that are emitted in short succession, also known as double clicks (Kothari et al., 2014Figure 1). Kothari et al. show that by using the double clicks to focus on a nearby object, the activity in the brain shifted. The neurons in the superior colliculus fired more frequently when close objects were inspected and registered the distance to them in better detail. This implies that the representation of the spatial surroundings in the midbrain becomes more precise.

The double clicking was also associated with an increase of electrical activity at high frequencies (between about 50 and 140 Hertz) in the midbrain. This 'gamma activity' is known to emerge when groups of neurons are involved in attention tasks (Fries, 2009). For example, gamma activity in the superior colliculus of barn owls increases when the birds focus their attention on specific objects (Sridharan and Knudsen, 2015), which seems to be similar to what happens in bats.

Overall, the work of Kothari et al. offers us a unique view on the bat’s sensory world as it flies through the dark. It shows for the first time that these animals create an acoustic representation of the external world in a region in the brain that specifically controls fast behavioural reactions to objects and obstacles. It also establishes bats as a promising model to study the mechanisms of sensory attention and the interaction between sensory and motors systems.

References

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Manfred Kössl

    Manfred Kössl is in the Institute for Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Goethe University, Frankfurt/Main, Germany

    For correspondence
    koessl@bio.uni-frankfurt.de
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  2. Julio Hechavarría

    Julio Hechavarría is in the Institute for Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Goethe University, Frankfurt/Main, Germany

    For correspondence
    hechavarria@bio.uni-frankfurt.de
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-9277-2339

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published:

Copyright

© 2018, Kössl et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 3,180
    views
  • 123
    downloads
  • 0
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Manfred Kössl
  2. Julio Hechavarría
(2018)
Echolocation: Smart bats click twice
eLife 7:e36561.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36561

Further reading

  1. The techniques used by bats to navigate space in three dimensions are becoming clearer.

    1. Neuroscience
    Katie Morris, Edita Bulovaite ... Mathew H Horrocks
    Research Article

    The concept that dimeric protein complexes in synapses can sequentially replace their subunits has been a cornerstone of Francis Crick’s 1984 hypothesis, explaining how long-term memories could be maintained in the face of short protein lifetimes. However, it is unknown whether the subunits of protein complexes that mediate memory are sequentially replaced in the brain and if this process is linked to protein lifetime. We address these issues by focusing on supercomplexes assembled by the abundant postsynaptic scaffolding protein PSD95, which plays a crucial role in memory. We used single-molecule detection, super-resolution microscopy and MINFLUX to probe the molecular composition of PSD95 supercomplexes in mice carrying genetically encoded HaloTags, eGFP, and mEoS2. We found a population of PSD95-containing supercomplexes comprised of two copies of PSD95, with a dominant 12.7 nm separation. Time-stamping of PSD95 subunits in vivo revealed that each PSD95 subunit was sequentially replaced over days and weeks. Comparison of brain regions showed subunit replacement was slowest in the cortex, where PSD95 protein lifetime is longest. Our findings reveal that protein supercomplexes within the postsynaptic density can be maintained by gradual replacement of individual subunits providing a mechanism for stable maintenance of their organization. Moreover, we extend Crick’s model by suggesting that synapses with slow subunit replacement of protein supercomplexes and long-protein lifetimes are specialized for long-term memory storage and that these synapses are highly enriched in superficial layers of the cortex where long-term memories are stored.