Associative learning drives longitudinally graded presynaptic plasticity of neurotransmitter release along axonal compartments
Abstract
Anatomical and physiological compartmentalization of neurons is a mechanism to increase the computational capacity of a circuit, and a major question is what role axonal compartmentalization plays. Axonal compartmentalization may enable localized, presynaptic plasticity to alter neuronal output in a flexible, experience-dependent manner. Here, we show that olfactory learning generates compartmentalized, bidirectional plasticity of acetylcholine release that varies across the longitudinal compartments of Drosophila mushroom body (MB) axons. The directionality of the learning-induced plasticity depends on the valence of the learning event (aversive vs. appetitive), varies linearly across proximal to distal compartments following appetitive conditioning, and correlates with learning-induced changes in downstream mushroom body output neurons (MBONs) that modulate behavioral action selection. Potentiation of acetylcholine release was dependent on the CaV2.1 calcium channel subunit cacophony. In addition, contrast between the positive conditioned stimulus and other odors required the inositol triphosphate receptor, which maintained responsivity to odors upon repeated presentations, preventing adaptation. Downstream from the MB, a set of MBONs that receive their input from the γ3 MB compartment were required for normal appetitive learning, suggesting that they represent a key node through which reward learning influences decision-making. These data demonstrate that learning drives valence-correlated, compartmentalized, bidirectional potentiation, and depression of synaptic neurotransmitter release, which rely on distinct mechanisms and are distributed across axonal compartments in a learning circuit.
Editor's evaluation
This manuscript will be of interest to scientists working on learning and memory and synaptic plasticity. The study mostly uses an acetylcholine sensor in the fly brain to image activity, which is novel and helps to tie together previous studies reporting memory-induced changes in calcium transients. In particular, the study highlights the compartmentalised plasticity along Kenyon cell axon terminals, the main cell type of the insect mushroom body.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.76712.sa0Introduction
Neuronal dendrites carry out computations through compartmentalized signaling, while axons have long been considered to carry signals to their terminal fields relatively uniformly following spike initiation. However, anatomical and physiological compartmentalization of axons has been recently documented in neurons from worms through mammals (Boto et al., 2014; Cohn et al., 2015; Hendricks et al., 2012; Rowan et al., 2016). How axonal compartmentalization influences information flow across neuronal circuits and modulates behavioral outcomes is not understood. One functional role for axonal compartmentalization may be to enable localized, presynaptic plasticity to alter output from select axon compartments in a flexible, experience-dependent manner. This would vastly enhance the neuron’s flexibility and computational capabilities. A potential function of such compartmentalization would allow independent modulation of axonal segments and/or synaptic release sites by biologically salient events, such as sensory stimuli that drive learning.
The anatomical organization of the Drosophila mushroom body (MB) makes it an exemplary test bed to study how sensory information is processed during learning and rerouted to alter behavioral outcomes. The MB encodes odor in sparse representations across the intrinsic MB neurons, Kenyon cells (KCs), which are arranged in several parallel sets. They project axons in fasciculated bundles into several anatomically distinct, but spatially adjacent lobes (α/β, α′/β′, and γ) (Crittenden et al., 1998). KC axons are longitudinally subdivided into discrete tiled compartments (Aso et al., 2014a). Each compartment receives afferent neuromodulatory input from unique dopaminergic neurons (Aso et al., 2014a; Mao and Davis, 2009), and innervates unique efferent mushroom body output neurons (MBONs) (Aso et al., 2014a). Each set of dopaminergic neurons plays an individual role in learning, with some modulating aversive learning (Mao and Davis, 2009; Schroll et al., 2006; Schwaerzel et al., 2003), others modulating reward learning (Liu et al., 2012; Yamagata et al., 2015), and a third class modulating memory strength without driving behavioral valence (Boto et al., 2019). Similarly, each MBON has a unique effect on behavioral approach and avoidance, with some biasing the animal to approach, others biasing the animal to avoidance, and some having no known effect (Aso et al., 2014b; Perisse et al., 2016; Plaçais et al., 2013; Séjourné et al., 2011).
A major question in learning and memory is how presynaptic plasticity contributes to reweighting the flow of sensory signals across each of the downstream ‘approach’ or ‘avoidance’ circuits, altering action selection and memory retrieval. In naïve conditions, Drosophila dopaminergic circuits modulate cAMP in a compartmentalized fashion along the MB axons (Boto et al., 2014). This compartmentalized dopaminergic signaling can independently modulate Ca2+ responses in each compartment, as well as the responses of the downstream valence-coding MBONs (Cohn et al., 2015). Presynaptic plasticity within each KC compartment likely contributes to the changes in downstream MBON activity that guide learned behavioral responses (Zhang et al., 2019). However, manipulation of the ‘aversive’ protocerebral posterior lateral 1 (PPL1) dopaminergic neurons does not detectably alter Ca2+ signals in KCs (Boto et al., 2019; Hige et al., 2015a). Furthermore, Ca2+ responses in KCs are uniformly potentiated across compartments with appetitive classical conditioning protocols and unaltered in KCs following aversive protocols (Louis et al., 2018). This raises the question of how local, compartmentalized synaptic plasticity in KCs drives coherent changes across the array of downstream MBONs to modulate action selection during memory retrieval. Learning/dopamine-induced plasticity has been demonstrated in the MBONs (Berry et al., 2018; Hige et al., 2015a; Hige et al., 2015b; Owald et al., 2015), with dopamine also acting directly on them (Takemura et al., 2017) (in addition to KCs). Feedforward inhibition among MBONs that drive opposing behavioral outcomes provides a mechanism explaining how bidirectional valence coding in MBONs could be generated, with or without bidirectional presynaptic plasticity (Perisse et al., 2016). The compartmentalized, dopamine-dependent plasticity in KCs and the necessity for dopamine receptors and downstream signaling molecules in the intrinsic KCs points to a potential presynaptic contribution (Kim et al., 2007; McGuire et al., 2003; Zars et al., 2000). Thus, compartmentalized presynaptic plasticity could contribute to reweighting the flow of olfactory information to downstream circuits.
Here, we describe how learning alters the flow of information through the MB via alteration of synaptic release of the KC neurotransmitter acetylcholine (ACh) (Barnstedt et al., 2016), using a genetically encoded indicator of ACh neurotransmission. The data reveal that appetitive and aversive learning alter compartmentalized acetylcholine release in distinct spatial patterns, with differing molecular mechanisms, coherently reweighting the flow of olfactory information across the ensemble of downstream neurons that mediate action selection.
Results
Associative learning modulates neurotransmitter release in a spatially distinct manner across longitudinal axonal compartments
Synapses within each MB compartment transmit olfactory information from KCs to compartment-specific MBONs (Figure 1, Figure 5 A, Figure 5-figuresupplement 1A ; Aso et al., 2014a; Tanaka et al., 2008). The MBONs exert distinct and often-opposing effects on behavior, with some innately promoting approach and others promoting avoidance (Aso et al., 2014b; Berry et al., 2018; Ichinose et al., 2015; Owald et al., 2015; Perisse et al., 2016; Plaçais et al., 2013; Sayin et al., 2019; Séjourné et al., 2011). Synaptic depression has been observed (postsynaptically) at KC-MBON synapses following pairing of odor with stimulation of PPL1 neurons that are critical for aversive learning (Hige et al., 2015a), suggesting that depression could be the primary learning rule implemented at these synapses. While some MBONs exhibit bidirectional responses to conditioning, the major described mechanism involves a sign change that occurs postsynaptic to the KCs (polysynaptic feedforward inhibition) (Owald et al., 2015; Perisse et al., 2016) and the presynaptic contributions remain unknown. To test for the presence, directionality, and variation of presynaptic plasticity across MB axonal compartments, we expressed a synaptic ACh sensor to monitor neurotransmitter release from KCs in vivo (Zhang et al., 2019). The genetically encoded ACh reporter, GPCR-Activation–Based-ACh sensor (GRAB-ACh) (Figure 1A; Jing et al., 2020; Jing et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2019), was expressed in KCs using the broad KC driver 238Y-Gal4. Appetitive conditioning was carried out, monitoring ACh release from the γ lobe compartments evoked by the olfactory conditioned stimuli (CS+ and CS-) before and after pairing the CS+ with a sucrose unconditioned stimulus (US) (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Responses were compared to those in odor-only control cohorts to determine whether any learning-induced changes resulted from potentiation or depression. We quantified several parameters (Figure 1—figure supplement 1), starting with how the response to each odor changed after conditioning (e.g. Figure 1D and F). This was collapsed to a single value, the within-treatment Δ(post/pre), for comparison across conditions. The Δ(post/pre) of the CS+ and CS- was compared to determine how each changed relative to the other, and then each was compared to its respective odor-only control to quantify whether it was potentiated or depressed, accounting for any non-associative olfactory adaptation (Figure 1G-K, Figure 1—figure supplement 2).
Appetitive conditioning produced plasticity in ACh release that varied across the axonal compartments of the MB γ lobe in several key ways (Figure 1). Conditioning significantly increased CS+ responses relative to the CS- responses in the three most proximal γ lobe compartments: γ1, γ2, and γ3 (Figure 1C, D–; Figure 1—figure supplement 2). In each of these compartments, this was due to different underlying dynamics. In the γ1 compartment, the CS+response was potentiated: following appetitive conditioning, the Δ(post/pre) CS+ response was significantly larger than the respective odor-only control (ethyl butyrate: EB), while the CS- did not differ from its odor-only control (isoamyl acetate: IA) (Figure 1G, Figure 1—figure supplement 2). In γ1, the CS+ response significantly increased following conditioning (Figure 1D). While the CS- response decreased following conditioning (Figure 1D), this decrease was indistinguishable from the rate of adaptation among odor-only controls in that compartment (Figure 1G). This suggests that the main effect in the γ1 compartment was potentiation of the CS+ response, and highlights the value of normalizing for non-associative olfactory adaptation. In the γ2 compartment, both the CS+ was potentiated and the CS- depressed relative to the odor-only controls (Figure 1H, Figure 1—figure supplement 2). In the γ3 compartment, while the CS+ and CS- differed, neither was significantly altered relative to odor-only controls. However, there was a trend toward depression in the CS- group (P = 0.099) (Figure 1I), as well as a significant decrease in the post-conditioning CS- response (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Overall, these data reveal a spatial gradient of relative CS+ enhancement in the proximal γ compartments, shifting from CS+ potentiation in γ1 toward CS- depression in γ3, with the spatially intermediate γ2 exhibiting both (Figure 1L). This gradient of CS+:CS- plasticity suggests that both the CS+ and CS- contribute to learning by modulating compartmentalized KC (and downstream MBON) output, with different forms of plasticity in each compartment.
Moving further down the distal length of the KC axons, the plasticity produced by appetitive conditioning shifted from relative CS+ potentiation (↑CS+:CS-) to depression. In the most distal γ5 compartment, the CS+ response was reduced relative to the CS- following appetitive conditioning (Figure 1E, F, K, Figure 1—figure supplement 2). The effect could not be unambiguously assigned to CS+ depression, though there was no evidence of CS- potentiation (Figure 1K, Figure 1—figure supplement 2). In addition, control flies for RNAi experiments (discussed further below) exhibited a trend toward a reduction in the CS+:CS- response in the γ5 compartment following appetitive conditioning (Figure 3), accompanied by a significant decrease in CS+ response (Figure 3—figure supplement 1D). These lines of evidence point to a depression in CS+ evoked ACh release from the distal γ5 compartment. Overall, appetitive conditioning increased relative CS+responsivity in γ1–γ3 compartments, which was derived from a proximal-to-distal gradient of CS+ potentiation to CS- depression, and reduction of relative CS+ responsivity in the γ5 compartment (Figure 1L). Thus, the plasticity was bidirectional between the proximal and distal axonal compartments. This likely contributes to approach behavior by simultaneously enhancing the conditioned odor-evoked activation of downstream ‘approach’ circuits and inhibiting ‘avoidance’ MBON circuits.
Aversive conditioning drives ACh release in the opposite direction across MB compartments
The above data suggested that appetitive conditioning potentiated ACh synaptic release from the proximal γ lobe compartments. Yet synaptic depression is the main described plasticity mechanism at the KC-MBON synapses following olfactory conditioning (Barnstedt et al., 2016; Modi et al., 2020; Owald et al., 2015; Perisse et al., 2016; Séjourné et al., 2011; Zhang and Roman, 2013; Zhang et al., 2019). In the γ1 compartment, where it has been examined in detail with electrophysiology, aversive reinforcement via dopaminergic neuron stimulation produces synaptic depression (Hige et al., 2015a). Since many of the prior studies involved aversive reinforcement, we reasoned that appetitive and aversive conditioning may produce bidirectional plasticity of ACh release within each compartment, with the sign/directionality matching the postsynaptic MBON valence. To test this, we examined whether aversive conditioning produced the opposite effect in the same compartments as appetitive conditioning did. ACh release from KCs was imaged with GRAB-ACh and flies were trained with an aversive odor-shock conditioning protocol (Figure 2A and B). In these experiments, we focused on the γ2–γ5 compartments, as the fly was mounted at a higher angle, making the GRAB-ACh signal difficult to simultaneously visualize from γ1 along with that of the other compartments. Following aversive conditioning, there was a reduction in the CS+ response relative to the CS- in the γ2 and γ3 compartments (Figure 2C-H, Figure 2—figure supplement 1). This was due to depression in the CS+ response, as the Δ(post/pre) of the CS+ was significantly lower than the odor-only controls in each compartment (Figure 2G and H). The γ4 and γ5 compartments exhibited no significant change in ACh release (Figure 2I, J and Figure 2—figure supplement 1). When compared to appetitive conditioning, aversive conditioning produced plasticity in the opposite direction in the γ2 and γ3 compartments (Figures 1L and 2K). Thus, appetitive and aversive conditioning produced bidirectional plasticity within multiple compartments, which likely represents a presynaptic contribution to learning-induced changes in odor responsivity among postsynaptic MBONs following conditioning (Berry et al., 2018; Hige et al., 2015a; Owald et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2019).
Presynaptic potentiation relies on the acophony CaV2.1 Ca2+ channel
Associative learning alters Ca2+ transients in MB γ neurons (Louis et al., 2018), which could influence neurotransmitter release. Major sources of stimulus-evoked intracellular Ca2+ include influx through voltage-sensitive CaV2 channels, which are involved in presynaptic short-term and homeostatic plasticity (Frank et al., 2006; Inchauspe et al., 2004; Ishikawa et al., 2005; Müller and Davis, 2012). To probe the Ca2+-dependent molecular mechanisms underlying presynaptic plasticity, we first knocked down the α subunit of the CaV2 Ca2+ channel encoded by cacophony (Cac). Cac was knocked down conditionally in adult MBs with RNAi using the strong, KC-selective R13F02-Gal4 driver, combined with the ubiquitous temperature-sensitive tub-Gal80ts repressor (McGuire et al., 2003) to reduce the potential for developmental effects (Figure 3A). The RNAi line was selected to moderately reduce, but not eliminate, Cac expression (Brusich et al., 2015). Quantitative reverse transcription polymerase chain reaction (RT-qPCR) analysis of Cac knockdown (driven ubiquitously with tubulin-Gal4) showed that conditional expression with this induction protocol reduced Cac levels ~29% (Figure 3—figure supplement 1C). For imaging experiments, RNAi expression was induced in the MB with R13F02-Gal4 following eclosion, and ACh release from KCs was imaged with GRAB-ACh (Jing et al., 2020; Jing et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2019). Control flies (containing R13F02-Gal4, UAS-GRAB-ACh, and tub-Gal80ts, but lacking a UAS-RNAi) exhibited plasticity across the γ lobe in similar spatial patterns as observed in wild-type animals (↑CS+:CS- in γ1–3 and ↓CS+:CS- in γ5): there was a robust increase in the CS+:CS- and CS+ potentiation in γ1, with a strong trend toward an increase in CS+:CS- in γ2 (p = 0.052), an increase in the CS+ response in γ3, and a trend toward a CS+ decrease in γ5 (p = 0.120) (Figure 3C, E, F, Figure 3—figure supplement 1). When Cac was knocked down conditionally, odor-evoked ACh release was still observed, demonstrating that synaptic exocytosis remained intact. Yet the CS+ potentiation was lost across the γ1–γ3 compartments (Figure 3D, E, G, Figure 3—figure supplement 1). This demonstrates that the presynaptic CaV2.1 channel is necessary for potentiation of ACh release induced by learning. Further, this CaV-dependent potentiation may underlie behavioral learning, a possibility we explore below.
Data from the appetitive conditioning experiments suggested that potentiation of the CS+ response was dependent on the voltage-sensitive CaV2 Ca2+ channel Cac. Interestingly, the trend toward CS+ depression in the most distal γ5 compartment remained intact when Cac was knocked down (Figure 3G and J). This suggests that while presynaptic potentiation requires Cac across the MB compartments, depression may not. To directly test whether depression of the CS+ was affected, we turned to aversive conditioning, which generates robust CS+ depression in the proximal γ compartments (Figure 2). Cac was knocked down using the same conditional RNAi strategy as above. Control flies for conditional Cac knockdown experiments exhibited similar CS+ depression in the proximal γ2 and γ3 compartments following aversive conditioning (Figure 4A-C, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Knockdown of Cac did not appreciably impair depression of CS+ responses (Figure 4D-F, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). There was a significant depression in γ2, both in terms of the drop in CS+ following conditioning (Figure 4D, Figure 4—figure supplement 1) and when comparing the Δ(post/pre) of the CS+ to either the CS- or the odor-only control (EB) (Figure 4E, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). In the γ1 compartment, there was a trend toward a decrease in the CS+ response compared to the CS- that matched the controls (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). In γ3, the difference between the CS+ and CS- (or CS+ vs odor-only control) did not reach significance; yet there was a trend in the same direction as the controls and the CS+ response significantly depressed following conditioning (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Overall, these data suggest that moderate knockdown of Cac does not affect learning-induced depression of ACh release (in contrast to potentiation).
Reduction of Cac in KCs impairs behavioral reward learning
Reducing Cac levels in KCs altered the pattern of plasticity in ACh release across MB compartments following conditioning, impairing potentiation following appetitive conditioning. This represents a potential physiological mechanism underlying reward learning. To test whether reduction in Cac levels impaired reward learning, we carried out behavioral olfactory appetitive conditioning experiments. Cac was knocked down in KCs using the same conditional expression strategy as above, reducing Cac level ~29% (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Flies were trained with a conditioning paradigm in which a CS+ (EB or IA) was paired with sucrose, the other odor was presented as the CS-, and the flies were allowed to choose between the two odors in a T-maze. Conditional Cac knockdown impaired behavioral performance (Figure 3H), reminiscent of its previously reported role in aversive conditioning (Hidalgo et al., 2021). Odor avoidance was unaffected by Cac knockdown (Figure 3—figure supplement 1A,B), demonstrating that the effect was not due to loss of olfactory acuity. Overall, these findings suggest that the learning-induced facilitation of neurotransmitter release in the proximal γ compartments contributes to behavioral action selection in reward learning.
Post-conditioning odor contrast and maintenance of odor responses are dependent on IP3 Signaling
Ca2+ release from the endoplasmic reticulum (ER) is a major source of stimulus-evoked Ca2+ in neurons, including KCs, and modulates various forms of synaptic/homeostatic plasticity (Handler et al., 2019; James et al., 2019; Taufiq et al., 2005). Therefore, we reasoned that inositol triphosphate receptor (IP3R) mediated Ca2+ release may contribute to presynaptic plasticity across MB compartments. To test this, we conditionally knocked down the IP3R in the adult MB with RNAi. GRAB-ACh was expressed in the MB (as above), while conditionally knocking down IP3R (Figure 4, Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Figure 4—figure supplement 2). For these experiments, flies were aversively conditioned (IP3R knockdown impairs feeding under the microscope, precluding appetitive conditioning). Knockdown of IP3R eliminated the post-conditioning contrast between the CS+ and CS- (i.e. the difference between the CS+ and CS-) (Figure 4G-I, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). However, this was not due to a loss of depression in the CS+ groups, rather it was due to the appearance of olfactory adaptation in the other groups. This was seen as a decrease in the post-treatment odor responses in the CS- and odor-only controls (Figure 4G, Figure 4—figure supplement 1C), with concomitant Δ(post/pre) values < 1 (Figure 4H, Figure 4—figure supplement 1D). As there was no US presented in the odor-only control groups, this reduction in olfactory response represents adaptation, rather than a conditioned, associative change. Therefore, in normal conditions, release of Ca2+ from the ER via IP3R is necessary to maintain odor responsivity upon repeated odor presentations. Loss of IP3R renders the KCs more susceptible to adaptation at this time point (Figure 1—figure supplement 1, Figure 4H), reducing the contrast between the CS+ (which exhibits depression following aversive learning) and the other odor(s).
IP3R maintains neurotransmitter release during repeated odor exposure
To directly test how adaptation to odors was impacted by the loss of IP3R, as suggested by analysis of pre/post odor responses in the conditioning experiments above, we carried out an olfactory adaptation assay. Flies were presented an odor 10 times over the course of 10 min while imaging the GRAB-ACh responses. To test for olfactory adaptation at each time point, the ΔF/F was compared to the initial naïve odor presentation (Figure 4J, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Control animals exhibited no significant olfactory adaptation across the 10 trials in any compartment (Figure 4K, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). In contrast, knocking down IP3R in KCs produced significant olfactory adaptation in the γ2 and γ3 compartments (Figure 4L, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). In γ2, following the fourth-odor presentation, there was a significant depression in odor-evoked ACh release at all-time points save one (Figure 4L, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). A similar effect was observed in the γ3, where following the third-odor presentation, a significant depression in release occurred (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). The γ1 compartment showed no evidence of adaptation, suggesting that IP3R may support plasticity in this compartment via another mechanism (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). The γ4 and γ5 compartments exhibited no significant adaptation (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). Overall, these data suggest that IP3R-dependent Ca2+ release from the ER may contribute to maintenance of synaptic strength, preventing adaptation during repeated stimuli. Further, loss of IP3R leads to alterations in neurotransmitter release that particularly impact compartments dominated by CS+depression.
To confirm the site of ACh release, we blocked neurotransmitter release from KCs by expressing the temperature-sensitive dynamin mutant Shibirets with R13F02-Gal4 (Kitamoto, 2001; McGuire et al., 2001). Odor-evoked GRAB-ACh transients were imaged across MB compartments, as above, before, and after shifting flies from the permissive (20°C) to the restrictive (30°C) temperature. Blocking synaptic transmission from KCs inhibited the GRAB-ACh responses across compartments (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). There was some residual response at the restrictive temperature (particularly in γ1), which we reasoned may emanate from KCs that were not labeled by the R13F02-Gal4 driver. To quantify the coverage of this driver, we counted GRAB-ACh labeled neurons (immunostained with an anti-GFP antibody), finding that R13F02-Gal4 labeled 745 ± 47 neurons (Figure 4—figure supplement 3) out of ~2000 total KCs (Aso et al., 2009). Inhibiting this relatively small subset of KCs significantly reduced the GRAB-ACh response from the MB compartments, demonstrating that KCs are the major ACh source, though cholinergic MBONs could also contribute to the signal (Scheffer et al., 2020).
Compartmentalized plasticity propagates into downstream mushroom body output neurons
Since ACh release from each compartment provides input to unique postsynaptic MBONs, the presynaptic plasticity observed in each compartment should be mirrored in the respective postsynaptic MBON(s) innervating that compartment. To test this, we imaged Ca2+ responses in MBONs with GCaMP and examined the effect of appetitive conditioning. Four sets of MBONs were tested, each innervating and receiving cholinergic input from a distinct MB γ lobe compartment: γ1pedc>α/β, γ2α′1, γ3/γ3β′1, and γ5β′2a (Figure 5A). Within the γ lobe, these neurons innervate the γ1, γ2, γ3, and γ5 compartments, respectively (Figure 5B–F). We focused our analysis on the γ2α′1 and γ3/γ3β′1 MBONs, which have not been studied intensively in the context of appetitive conditioning. The γ2α′1 MBON exhibited an increase in the relative CS+ responses following conditioning (Figure 5D). The plasticity could not be unambiguously attributed to purely CS+ potentiation or CS- depression. The γ3/γ3β′1 MBONs exhibited an increase in the relative CS+ responses (Figure 5F). In this case, it was due to potentiation of the CS+ response (Figure 5F). Note that this pair of MBONs is not parsed with available drivers and they were imaged together. One of them (γ3β′1) receives major input from the β′1 lobe/compartment in addition to γ3. Presynaptically, the γ3 compartment exhibited a depression in the CS- response, suggesting that the potentiation in the γ3/γ3β′1 CS+ response may emanate from potentiated β′1 inputs. The γ1pedc>α/β and γ5β′2 a MBONs have been reported to exhibit bidirectional plasticity following aversive and appetitive conditioning (Owald et al., 2015; Perisse et al., 2016); we examined the plasticity in these MBONs (and others) using the same odors/conditions that were used to probe presynaptic plasticity in KCs. Following appetitive conditioning, the γ1pedc>α/β MBON exhibited a significant elevation of the CS+ response relative to the CS- (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). This was due to a potentiation of the CS+ response, as the post-conditioning CS+ response was significantly larger than the corresponding odor-only control. Finally, appetitive conditioning produced plasticity in the opposite direction in the γ5β′2 a MBON; this neuron exhibited a decrease in the CS+ response relative to the CS- (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Overall, the directionality of the plasticity in MBONs matched that observed in ACh responses in the presynaptic compartment. Thus, compartmentalized, presynaptic plasticity in neurotransmitter release from the MB compartments likely plays a role in modulating the MBON responses following learning.
Synaptic activity from the γ3-innervating MBONs mediates reward learning
The unique role of the γ2 and γ3 compartments in encoding CS- plasticity, as well as their strong plasticity following both appetitive and aversive conditioning, led us to question the behavioral roles of the MBONs that receive input from these compartments (Figures 1 and 5A). In particular, the involvement of these MBONs in appetitive memory is unclear. To test whether the MBONs innervating the γ2 and γ3 compartments mediate short-term appetitive memory, we carried out behavioral appetitive classical conditioning, blocking synaptic transmission from MBONs with Shibirets (Shits) (Kitamoto, 2001; McGuire et al., 2001; Figure 5A and B). Blocking the γ2α′1 MBON did not significantly impair performance in appetitive conditioning (Figure 5B). Therefore, while activation of the γ2α′1 MBON drives approach behavior (Aso et al., 2014b) and the neuron is necessary for aversive memory (Berry et al., 2018), it is not crucial for appetitive short-term memory in the otherwise intact nervous system. In contrast, blocking synaptic transmission from the γ3/γ3β′1 MBONs significantly impaired appetitive conditioning performance (Figure 5B). This demonstrates that the output of the γ3/γ3β′1 MBONs is not only sufficient for protein synthesis-dependent appetitive long-term memory (Wu et al., 2017) but also serves a key role for normal appetitive short-term memory. These neurons convey the output of the MB γ3 compartment to the crepine and superior medial protocerebrum (where they innervate interneurons that project to the fan-shaped body and lateral accessory lobe further downstream), as well as provide direct contralateral MB feedback and form polysynaptic feedback loops via MB-innervating PAM dopaminergic neurons and other MBONs (Scaplen et al., 2021; Xu et al., 2020). The γ3 PAM dopaminergic neuron is also bidirectionally modulated through sucrose-activated allatostatin A neurons (Yamagata et al., 2016). These multi-layered connections provide several routes through which they could modulate behavioral output following learning. Overall, the present data suggest that the γ3/γ3β′1 MBONs receive input from an MB compartment with unique physiology and represent a key node through which discriminative effects influence sucrose-activated appetitive memory and decision-making.
Discussion
Compartmentalized plasticity in neurotransmitter release expands the potential computational capacity of learning circuits. It allows a set of odor-coding MB neurons to bifurcate their output to different downstream approach- and avoidance-driving downstream output neurons, independently modulating the synaptic connections to alter action selection based on the conditioned value of olfactory stimuli. The KCs modify the encoded value of olfactory stimuli through bidirectional plasticity in odor responses, which vary in a compartment-specific manner along the length of the axons. These changes were observed following pairing an olfactory CS with gustatory/somatosensory US (sucrose feeding or electric shock) in vivo. The CS+ and CS- drive unique patterns of plasticity in each compartment, demonstrating that olfactory stimuli are reweighted differently across compartments following learning, depending on the temporal associations of the stimuli. Different molecular mechanisms govern the potentiation of trained odor responses (CaV2/Cac) and maintenance of responsivity over time (IP3R). Finally, one set of γ output neurons, γ3/γ3β′1, is important for appetitive short-term memory.
Learning-induced plasticity of ACh release in the MB was bidirectional within the compartment, depending on the valence of the US, and was coherent with the valence of the MBON downstream of the compartment. Notably, the γ2 and γ3 MB compartments, which relay information to approach-promoting MBONs (Aso et al., 2014b), exhibited plasticity that was coherent with promoting behavioral approach following appetitive conditioning and avoidance after aversive conditioning. There was an increase in the relative CS+:CS- ACh responses after appetitive conditioning, and conversely reduced CS+:CS- ACh responses following aversive conditioning. For this study, we focused on the time point 5 min following conditioning, which is consistent with behavioral short-term memory. Aversive conditioning was previously reported to decrease neurotransmitter release from KCs (Zhang and Roman, 2013; Zhang et al., 2019). Indirect evidence, via Ca2+ imaging in presynaptic KCs, suggested that increases in presynaptic neurotransmission could also be associated with learning. Pairing odor with stimulation of appetitive PAM dopaminergic neurons potentiates odor-evoked cytosolic Ca2+ transients across the KC compartments (Boto et al., 2014). Appetitive conditioning increases odor-evoked Ca2+ transients across KC compartments (Louis et al., 2018). Stimulation of dopaminergic circuits associated with reward learning potentiate MB γ4 connections with the respective γ4 MBON (Handler et al., 2019). We did not detect a statistically significant effect in γ4 with appetitive or aversive classical conditioning, though the CS+ and CS- trended in the same direction as the adjacent γ5 compartment following conditioning. Overall, the present data demonstrate that there are bidirectional changes in neurotransmitter (ACh) release from MB compartments following appetitive vs aversive learning and provide a window into the spatial patterns of plasticity across compartments following associative learning.
Behavioral alterations following conditioning involve changes in responses among the MBONs. As the KCs provide presynaptic olfactory input to the MBONs, it was a logical a priori assumption that presynaptic plasticity in the KCs could be altered in a compartmental manner and contribute to the changes in MBON responses after conditioning. Yet data from previous Ca2+ imaging experiments have not completely supported this model. Compartmentalized effects have been observed in KCs with non-associative learning protocols (Cohn et al., 2015) and within the γ4 compartment following associative learning (Handler et al., 2019). In contrast, classical conditioning produces no compartmentalized differences in odor-evoked Ca2+ responses. Appetitive conditioning with odor + sucrose pairing increases odor-evoked cytosolic Ca2+ transients in KCs across the γ lobe compartments (Louis et al., 2018). Aversive conditioning produces no net change across the compartments (Louis et al., 2018), but alters synapse-specific Ca2+ responses at the individual bouton level (Bilz et al., 2020). If the compartmental effects of conditioning (observed with Ca2+ imaging) in KCs drove a proportional change in neurotransmitter release, both the approach- and avoidance-promoting MBONs would be simultaneously potentiated. Extracellular influx of Ca2+ through voltage-gated calcium channels is a primary driver of neurotransmitter release; however, there are multiple sources of Ca2+ in the cytosol that could contribute to the GCaMP signals (Grienberger and Konnerth, 2012). A major conclusion of the present study is that learning drives compartmentalized plasticity in neurotransmitter release that is coherent with the behavioral valence of the corresponding MBON.
At least two major molecular mechanisms govern the spatial patterns of plasticity across the MB compartments: a Cac-dependent CS+ potentiation and an IP3R-dependent maintenance of sensory responses over trials/time. This suggests that different sources of Ca2+ play different roles in regulating KC synaptic responses. Cac is the pore-forming subunit of the voltage-sensitive, presynaptic CaV2 Ca2+ channel in Drosophila. CaV2 channels regulate several forms of synaptic plasticity, including paired-pulse facilitation, homeostatic plasticity, and long-term potentiation (Frank et al., 2006; Inchauspe et al., 2004; Nanou et al., 2016). Our data suggest that these channels regulate the spatial patterns of learning-induced plasticity in the MB unidirectionally (from baseline), with Cac underlying potentiation but not depression. CaV2 channel activity is modulated by presynaptic calcium and G protein-coupled receptor activity (Zamponi and Currie, 2013), and channel localization in the active zone dynamically regulates synaptic strength (Gratz et al., 2019; Lübbert et al., 2019). Thus, Cac insertion into, or increased clustering within, the active zones may underlie learning-induced potentiation (e.g. in the γ1-γ2 compartments following appetitive conditioning). Conditional knockdown of Cac, which reduced Cac levels by ~29%, impaired this potentiation, likely by decreasing the number of available channels for modulation. Baseline stimulus-evoked neurotransmitter release was maintained during Cac knockdown, mediated either by the significant residual Cac expression or compensation by other intracellular Ca2+ channels/sources. In contrast to the Cac effect on potentiation, IP3R was necessary to maintain normal odor responsivity when odors were presented repeatedly across multiple trials (whether those were pre/post trials in the conditioning protocol or 10× odor presentations in the adaptation protocol). This is broadly consistent with the temporal role of IP3R in maintenance of presynaptic homeostatic potentiation at the neuromuscular junction (James et al., 2019). In addition, dopaminergic circuits associated with reward learning drive release of Ca2+ from the endoplasmic reticulum when activated with KCs in a backward pairing paradigm ex vivo, potentiating MB γ4 connections with the respective γ4 MBON (Handler et al., 2019). This is consistent with a role for ER calcium in positively regulating synaptic strength.
We observed potentiation and depression of ACh release across multiple MB compartments following conditioning, providing a presynaptic mechanism that potentially contributes to shaping conditioned MBON responses. Importantly, by comparing the CS+ and CS- responses to those of untrained odors, we ascribed plasticity to potentiation or depression (accounting for any non-associative olfactory adaptation) within each compartment. This is relevant for modeling efforts, where it has been unclear whether to include potentiation (along with depression) in the learning rule(s) at KC-MBON synapses (Abdelrahman et al., 2021; Bennett et al., 2021; Jiang and Litwin-Kumar, 2021; Springer and Nawrot, 2021). In addition, the experiments revealed an additional layer of spatial regulation in the γ1–γ3 compartments: a gradient of CS+ potentiation to CS- depression following appetitive conditioning. Specifically, the CS+/CS- relationship changed in a linear gradient down the γ1–γ3 compartments following appetitive conditioning. Appetitive conditioning increased CS+ responses in the γ1 compartment, while decreasing the CS- responses in the γ3 compartment. The γ2 compartment yielded a mix of these responses. These patterns of plasticity have the net effect of increasing the relative response to the CS+ odor (↑CS+:CS-). Since the MBONs postsynaptic to these compartments drive behavioral approach (Aso et al., 2014b), this would bias the animal to approach the CS+ if it encountered both odors simultaneously. Such a situation occurs at the choice point of a T-maze during retrieval in a classical conditioning assay. The CS+ and CS- produce different patterns of plasticity at different loci (e.g. γ1 vs γ3), which presumably coordinate to regulate behavior via temporal integration of the odor and US cues (Berry et al., 2018; Handler et al., 2019; Jacob and Waddell, 2020; König et al., 2018; Tanimoto et al., 2004; Tully and Quinn, 1985). The CS+ is temporally contiguous with the US, while the CS- is nonoverlapping. Therefore, the timing of CS/US pairing drives plasticity differently in each compartment. These patterns of plasticity presumably coordinate to regulate memory formation and action selection during retrieval. For instance, while the γ1–γ3 compartments exhibited ↑CS+:CS- following appetitive conditioning, the γ5 compartment exhibited plasticity in the opposite direction: decreasing the relative response to the CS+ odor (↓CS+:CS-). As the γ5 compartment is presynaptic to an avoidance-promoting MBON, this plasticity pattern would coherently contribute to biasing the animal toward CS+ approach (reducing CS+ avoidance). Thus, it would work in concert with the plasticity in γ1–γ3 to bias the animal toward behavioral approach. Overall, plasticity is regulated in each MB compartment individually by the timing of events and the valence of the US, with the changes coordinated across multiple compartments to coherently drive behavior.
Behaviorally, MBONs innervating the γ lobe variably drive behavioral approach or avoidance when stimulated (Aso et al., 2014b). Despite the approach-promoting valence of the γ2α′one and γ3/γ3β′1 MBONs, among them, only the γ3/γ3β′1 MBONs produced a loss-of-function phenotype in behavioral appetitive conditioning. This suggests that redundancy and/or different weighting across approach promoting MBONs renders the system resilient to silencing some of them. A previous study found effects of blocking the γ2α′1 MBONs, though not γ3/γ3β′1 MBONs, when blocking individual steps of memory processing (acquisition, retention, and/or retrieval) with a 1 hr appetitive memory protocol (Ichinose et al., 2021). This suggests that the different MBONs have differing roles across time, with some redundancy in appetitive processing. Blocking synaptic output of γ3/γ3β′1 MBONs reduced appetitive conditioning performance in our experiments immediately following conditioning, suggesting that these neurons play a specific role in appetitive short-term memory.
The present and previous studies suggest that alterations of MBON activity following learning are the product of both presynaptic and postsynaptic plasticity at the KC-MBON synapses, as well as feedforward inhibition (Hige et al., 2015a; Perisse et al., 2016; Pribbenow et al., 2021). Blocking synaptic output from KCs impairs the acquisition of appetitive memories (30–60 min after conditioning), suggesting a role for postsynaptic plasticity (Pribbenow et al., 2021; Schwaerzel et al., 2003). However, this does not rule out presynaptic plasticity, as blocking KC output (with R13F02) leaves signaling from reinforcing dopaminergic neurons partially intact (Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2017), which likely shapes the presynaptic KC responses via heterosynaptic plasticity. At the circuit level, polysynaptic inhibition can convert depression from select MB compartments into potentiation in MBONs following learning; in one established example, reduction of odor-evoked responses in the GABAergic γ1pedc MBON following aversive conditioning disinhibits the downstream γ5β′2 a MBON (Owald et al., 2015; Perisse et al., 2016).
KC-MBON synapses represent one node of learning-related plasticity, which is distributed across multiple sites during learning. Short-term memory-related plasticity has been observed in multiple olfactory neurons, such as the antennal lobes (Yu et al., 2004) and KCs (Louis et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2008). In addition, connectomics studies have revealed complex connectivity within and beyond the MB, which is a multi-layered network including circuit motifs that influence the propagation of information and generation of plasticity during learning. Such connections include recurrent feedback (Aso et al., 2014a; Ichinose et al., 2015; Li et al., 2020; Otto et al., 2020; Scaplen et al., 2021; Takemura et al., 2017; Zhao et al., 2018). Some of these recurrent connections are from cholinergic MBONs that synapse within the MB, which could have contributed to the ACh signals we observed in this study. For instance, the γ2α′1 MBON is a cholinergic MBON that sends ~6% of its output back to the γ lobe (Scheffer et al., 2020). Some of the recurrent connections are formed by dopaminergic neurons, such as the PAM γ4<γ1/y2 (Aso et al., 2014a; Li et al., 2020). In addition, reciprocal connections between KCs and dopaminergic neurons in the vertical lobes are necessary for memory retrieval (Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2017). This adds another layer of recurrent circuitry that may participate in reinforcement during associative learning (Aso et al., 2014b; Ichinose et al., 2015; Ichinose et al., 2021). Across these circuits, some neurons corelease several neurotransmitters and act on an array of postsynaptic receptors, which contribute to plasticity distributed across multiple sites (Aso et al., 2019; Barnstedt et al., 2016; Berry et al., 2012; Bielopolski et al., 2019; Cohn et al., 2015; Handler et al., 2019; Haynes et al., 2015; Keene et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2007; Liu and Davis, 2009; Pribbenow et al., 2021; Schroll et al., 2006; Séjourné et al., 2011; Silva et al., 2015; Wu et al., 2013; Zhou et al., 2019).
Overall, plasticity between KCs and MBONs may guide behavior through biasing network activation to alter action selection in a probabilistic manner. Appetitive conditioning drives compartmentalized, presynaptic plasticity in KCs that correlates with postsynaptic changes in MBONs that guide learned behaviors. Prior studies documented only depression at these synapses at short time points following conditioning (Hige et al., 2015a; Zhang and Roman, 2013; Zhang et al., 2019). Here, we observed both potentiation and depression in ACh release in the MB, suggesting that bidirectional presynaptic plasticity modulates learned behaviors. These bidirectional changes likely integrate with plasticity at downstream circuit nodes that also undergo learning-induced plasticity to produce network-level alterations in odor responses across the olfactory pathway following salient events. Thus, plasticity in ACh release from KCs functions to modulate responsivity to olfactory stimuli features across graded plasticity maps down the MB axons.
Materials and methods
Fly strains
Request a detailed protocolFlies were fed and maintained on a standard cornmeal agar food mixture on a 12:12 light:dark cycle. The 238Y-Gal4 driver was selected for maximal KC coverage (Aso et al., 2009) and high expression levels (Louis et al., 2018). The R13F02-Gal4 driver was selected for greater specificity in KCs (Jenett et al., 2012). MBON drivers were selected from the FlyLight and split-Gal4 collections (R12G04, MB077b, and MB083c) (Jenett et al., 2012; Pfeiffer et al., 2010). The γ5β′2 a LexA MBON driver was generated by Krystyna Keleman (Zhao et al., 2018). RNAi lines were obtained from the VDRC (Cac: 101478) (Dietzl et al., 2007) and TRiP collections (IP3R: 25937) (Perkins et al., 2015) and crossed into flies expressing R13F02-Gal4 and tub-Gal80ts (McGuire et al., 2003). Final experimental genotypes were: Cac (w;UAS-GRAB-ACh/UAS-Cac-RNAi;R13F02-Gal4/UAS-tub-Gal80ts) and IP3R (w;UAS-GRAB-ACh/UAS-tub-Gal80ts;R13F02-Gal4/UAS-IP3R-RNAi), compared to genetic controls (w;UAS-GRAB-ACh/+;R13F02-Gal4/UAS-tub-Gal80ts). For quantitative analysis of Cac knockdown, flies of the experimental genotype (w;UAS-Cac-RNAi/UAS-tub-Gal80ts;tub-Gal4/+) were compared to genetic controls (w;UAS-tub-Gal80ts/VIE-260B;tub-Gal4/+).
Fly preparation for in vivo imaging
Request a detailed protocolFlies were briefly anesthetized, placed in a polycarbonate imaging chamber (Tomchik, 2013), and fixed with myristic acid (Sigma-Aldrich). The proboscis was fixed in the retracted position, except for appetitive conditioning experiments (as noted below). A cuticle window was opened, and the fat and tracheal air sacs were carefully removed to allow optical access to the brain. The top of the chamber was filled with saline solution (103 mM NaCl, 3 mM MBl, 5 mM HEPES, 1.5 mM CaCl2, 4 mM MgCl2·6H2O, 26 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM NaH2PO4·H2O, 10 mM trehalose, 7 mM sucrose, and 10 mM glucose), which was perfused over the dorsal head/brain at 2 ml/min via a peristaltic pump.
In vivo imaging
Request a detailed protocolGRAB-ACh (Jing et al., 2020; Jing et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2019) was driven in the KCs, using the 238Y or R13F02 driver. Within the KCs, ROIs were drawn around five γ lobe compartments (γ1–5) within a single imaging plane for appetitive, and (γ2–5) for aversive. Imaging was performed with a Leica TCS SP8 confocal microscope utilizing appropriate laser lines and emission filter settings. Odors were delivered with an airstream for 1 s (60 ml/min flow rate) by directing the air flow with solenoid valves between an empty vial (air) to another containing 1 μl odorant spotted on filter paper. Odor-evoked responses were calculated as the baseline normalized change in fluorescence (ΔF/F), using the maximum ΔF/F within a 4 s window after odor delivery. In experiments with RNAi, flies containing the R13F02-Gal4, GRAB-ACh, a UAS-RNAi line, and tub-Gal80ts were constructed; flies were raised at 18°C until eclosion, then transferred to 32°C for 4–10 days prior to the experiment. Experiments were carried out at room temperature (23°C) for ACh imaging/conditioning. For Ca2+ imaging experiments, GCaMP6f was expressed in the MBONs using the R12G04 (γ1pedc), MB077b (γ2α′1), MB083c (γ3) Gal4/split-Gal4 drivers, or the VT014702-LexA (γ5β′2) driver. Regions of interest were selected in accordance with prior studies (Berry et al., 2018; Jacob and Waddell, 2020; Zhao et al., 2018). Experiments were carried out the same way as ACh imaging, except presenting a 3 s odor delivery.
Appetitive conditioning and imaging
Request a detailed protocolAppetitive conditioning was carried out as previously described (Louis et al., 2018). Flies were starved for a period of 18–24 hr prior to conditioning. One odor (the CS+; ethyl butyrate) was presented in conjunction with a paired sucrose (1 M, containing green food coloring) US, and a second odor (the CS-; isoamyl acetate) was presented 30 s later. Six 1 s odor pulses were presented during conditioning over a 30 s period, with a 5 s inter-pulse interval, to prevent desensitization of the reporter. In odor-only control cohorts, the sucrose US was omitted. During conditioning, the US was presented continuously for 30 s. Pre/post odor-evoked responses were imaged prior to and after the imaging protocol, using a 1 s (ACh imaging) or 3 s (Ca2+ imaging) odor pulse. During odor-evoked response imaging, proboscis extension was blocked utilizing a thin metal loop attached to a custom motorized micromanipulator. During conditioning, the proboscis was released, and the flies were presented sucrose through a metal pipette fed by a syringe pump controlled via a micro-controller (Arduino). To assess feeding, flies were monitored using a digital microscope (Vividia); sucrose ingestion was visually confirmed by the presence of green food coloring in the abdomen.
Aversive conditioning and imaging
Request a detailed protocolFlies were mounted in a polycarbonate imaging chamber such that the brain could be imaged while odors were delivered to the antennae and electric shocks delivered to the legs via a shock grid below the fly. Conditioning was carried out by pairing a CS+ odor with electric shocks as follows: 6 × 1 s odor pulses, with a 5 s inter-pulse interval, paired with 6 × 90 V electric shocks. This was followed 30 s later by presentation of 6 × 1 s pulses of the CS- odor with 5 s inter-pulse interval. Pre- and post-conditioning odor-evoked GRAB-ACh responses were imaged using a 1 s odor pulse. In each animal, either the CS+ or CS- odor was tested pre- and post-conditioning.
Olfactory adaptation protocol
Request a detailed protocolFlies were mounted in the imaging chamber and the proboscis was fixed in place with myristic acid to minimize brain movement. Odors were presented for a 1 s period in 1 min intervals for 10 min. Odor-evoked ACh responses were imaged, the peak ΔF/F of each response quantified, and the response at each time point was compared to the initial naïve response.
Behavioral appetitive conditioning
Request a detailed protocolAdult flies, 2–5 day old, were trained under dim red light at 75% relative humidity. Appetitive conditioning experiments were performed in animals starved 16–20 hr. Groups of ~60 flies were exposed for 2 min to an odor (the CS-), followed by 30 s of air and 2 min of another odor, the (the CS+), paired with a 2 M sucrose solution dried on filter paper, at 32°C for Shibirets blockade. The odors were ethyl butyrate and isoamyl acetate, adjusted so that naïve flies equally avoided the two odors (0.05–0.1%). Memory was tested by inserting the trained flies into a T-maze, in which they chose between an arm containing the CS+ odor and an arm containing the CS- odor. Odors were bubbled at 500 ml/min air flow rate. Flies were allowed to distribute for a 2 min choice period. The Performance Index (P.I.) was calculated as (flies in the CS- arm)-(flies in the CS+ arm)/(total flies in both arms). Odor avoidance was tested by putting groups of ~60 flies in a T-maze in which they chose between an arm containing an odor and an arm containing air; an Avoidance Index was calculated as above for the P.I.
Immunohistochemistry
Request a detailed protocolAbout 5–7-day old adult flies were dissected in 1% paraformaldehyde in S2 medium and processed as previously described (Jenett et al., 2012). Brains were incubated with primary antibodies for 3 hr at room temperature, followed by secondary antibodies for 3 hr at room temperature and 4 days at 4°C. Incubations were performed in blocking serum (3% normal goat serum). Brains were then stained with NeuroTrace 530/615 Red Fluorescent Nissl Stain (1:100, Invitrogen) and incubated at 4°C overnight, followed by mounting in Vectashield (Vector Laboratories) for imaging. The following antibodies were used: rabbit anti-GFP (1:1000, Invitrogen), mouse anti-brp (nc82) (1:50, DSHB), goat anti-rabbit IgG, and goat anti-mouse IgG (1:800, Alexa 488 or Alexa 633, respectively, Invitrogen). KCs were imaged using Leica TCS SP8 confocal microscope and LAS X software. Cells were counted using the point tool on Fiji 2.3.
Quantitative PCR
Request a detailed protocolPhenol-chloroform RNA extraction was performed with TRIzol (Ambion), and genomic DNA eliminated via DNase I (NEB). cDNA was generated using TaqMan RT Reagents (Applied Biosciences). RT-qPCR was performed using PowerUp SYBR Green Master Mix (Applied Biosciences) and an Applied Biosystems 7900HT Fast Real-Time PCR System. Samples were run in quadruplicate and raw Ct values extracted and averaged. Values for ΔCt were exported to Prism/GraphPad for significance testing. Knockdown efficiency was calculated from ΔΔCT values representing the fold change relative to the housekeeping gene. The following primers were used: Cacophony: 5′-GCAAGGCGAAGCTGAGTTAC-3′ (Forward), 5′-AGGCGTTGACACCACAATTC-3′ (Reverse) and Rps20: 5′-TGTGGTGAGGGTTCCAAGAC-3′ (Forward), 5′-GACGATCTCAGAGGGCGAGT-3′ (Reverse).
Quantification and statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolData were compared using Wilcoxon rank-sum tests (two groups, nonparametric), ANOVA/Sidak (multiple comparisons, parametric) or Kruskal–Wallis/Dunn (multiple comparisons, nonparametric) tests. Box plots show graph the median as a line, the 1st and 3rd quartile enclosed in the box, and whiskers extending from the 10th to the 90th percentile.
Data availability
Raw data have been deposited to Dryad under https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.dfn2z353h.
-
Dryad Digital RepositoryData from: Associative learning drives longitudinally-graded presynaptic plasticity of neurotransmitter release along axonal compartments.https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.dfn2z353h
References
-
The mushroom body of adult Drosophila characterized by GAL4 driversJournal of Neurogenetics 23:156–172.https://doi.org/10.1080/01677060802471718
-
A single-cross, RNA interference-based genetic tool for examining the long-term maintenance of homeostatic plasticityFrontiers in Cellular Neuroscience 9:107.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2015.00107
-
Tripartite mushroom body architecture revealed by antigenic markersLearning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.) 5:38–51.
-
Functional compensation of P/Q by N-type channels blocks short-term plasticity at the calyx of Held presynaptic terminalThe Journal of Neuroscience 24:10379–10383.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2104-04.2004
-
Models of heterogeneous dopamine signaling in an insect learning and memory centerPLOS Computational Biology 17:e1009205.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009205
-
A genetically encoded fluorescent acetylcholine indicator for in vitro and in vivo studiesNature Biotechnology 36:726–737.https://doi.org/10.1038/nbt.4184
-
Reinforcement signaling of punishment versus relief in fruit fliesLearning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.) 25:247–257.https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.047308.118
-
The role of Drosophila mushroom body signaling in olfactory memoryScience (New York, N.Y.) 293:1330–1333.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1062622
-
Spatiotemporal rescue of memory dysfunction in DrosophilaScience (New York, N.Y.) 302:1765–1768.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1089035
-
The Drosophila Mushroom Body: From Architecture to Algorithm in a Learning CircuitAnnual Review of Neuroscience 43:465–484.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-0621333
-
Dopamine and octopamine differentiate between aversive and appetitive olfactory memories in DrosophilaThe Journal of Neuroscience 23:10495–10502.
-
Muscarinic ACh Receptors Contribute to Aversive Olfactory Learning in DrosophilaNeural Plasticity 2015:658918.https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/658918
-
Neuronal assemblies of the Drosophila mushroom bodyThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 508:711–755.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21692
-
Involvement of IP3 receptors in LTP and LTD induction in guinea pig hippocampal CA1 neuronsLearning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.) 12:594–600.https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.17405
-
Dopaminergic neurons encode a distributed, asymmetric representation of temperature in DrosophilaThe Journal of Neuroscience 33:2166–76a.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3933-12.2013
-
Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogasterJournal of Comparative Physiology. A, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 157:263–277.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01350033
-
Imaging of an early memory trace in the Drosophila mushroom bodyThe Journal of Neuroscience 28:4368–4376.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2958-07.2008
-
Suppression of Dopamine Neurons Mediates RewardPLOS Biology 14:e1002586.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586
-
Regulation of Ca(V)2 calcium channels by G protein coupled receptorsBiochimica et Biophysica Acta 1828:1629–1643.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbamem.2012.10.004
-
Localization of a short-term memory in DrosophilaScience (New York, N.Y.) 288:672–675.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.288.5466.672
-
Aversive Training Induces Both Presynaptic and Postsynaptic Suppression in DrosophilaThe Journal of Neuroscience 39:9164–9172.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1420-19.2019
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institutes of Health (R01NS097237)
- Seth M Tomchik
National Institutes of Health (R01NS114403)
- Seth M Tomchik
National Institutes of Health (R00MH092294)
- Seth M Tomchik
Whitehall Foundation (2014-12-31)
- Seth M Tomchik
National Institutes of Health (R35NS097224)
- Ronald L Davis
Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission (Z181100001318002)
- Yulong Li
Beijing Brain Initiative of Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission (Z181100001518004)
- Yulong Li
Guangdong Grant "Key Technologies for Treatment of Brain Disorders" (2018B030332001)
- Yulong Li
General Program of National Natural Science Foundation of China Projects (31671118)
- Yulong Li
National Institutes of Health (NS103558)
- Yulong Li
General Program of National Natural Science Foundation of China Projects (31925017)
- Yulong Li
General Program of National Natural Science Foundation of China Projects (31871087)
- Yulong Li
The funders had no role in study design, data collection, and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
Fly stocks obtained from Krystyna Keleman and the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (NIH P40OD018537) were used in this study. The authors thank Yuexuan Li for the help in the development of the GRAB-ACh sensor, Brock Grill for helpful discussions, and Melissa Benilous for administrative assistance. Research support was provided by NIH R00MH092294 (S.M.T.), R01 NS097237 (S.M.T.), R01 NS114403 (S.M.T.), the Whitehall Foundation (S.M.T.), NIH R35NS097224 (R.L.D.), the Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission Z181100001318002 (Y.L.), the Beijing Brain Initiative of Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission Z181100001518004 (Y.L.), Guangdong Grant “Key Technologies for Treatment of Brain Disorders” 2018B030332001 (Y.L.), the General Program of National Natural Science Foundation of China projects 31671118, 31871087, and 31925017 (Y.L.), the NIH BRAIN Initiative NS103558 (Y.L.), grants from the Peking-Tsinghua Center for Life Sciences (Y.L.) and the State Key Laboratory of Membrane Biology at Peking University School of Life Sciences (Y.L.).
Copyright
© 2022, Stahl et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 2,255
- views
-
- 338
- downloads
-
- 23
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Motivation depends on dopamine, but might be modulated by acetylcholine which influences dopamine release in the striatum, and amplifies motivation in animal studies. A corresponding effect in humans would be important clinically, since anticholinergic drugs are frequently used in Parkinson’s disease, a condition that can also disrupt motivation. Reward and dopamine make us more ready to respond, as indexed by reaction times (RT), and move faster, sometimes termed vigour. These effects may be controlled by preparatory processes that can be tracked using electroencephalography (EEG). We measured vigour in a placebo-controlled, double-blinded study of trihexyphenidyl (THP), a muscarinic antagonist, with an incentivised eye movement task and EEG. Participants responded faster and with greater vigour when incentives were high, but THP blunted these motivational effects, suggesting that muscarinic receptors facilitate invigoration by reward. Preparatory EEG build-up (contingent negative variation [CNV]) was strengthened by high incentives and by muscarinic blockade, although THP reduced the incentive effect. The amplitude of preparatory activity predicted both vigour and RT, although over distinct scalp regions; frontal activity predicted vigour, whereas a larger, earlier, central component predicted RT. The incentivisation of RT was partly mediated by the CNV, though vigour was not. Moreover, the CNV mediated the drug’s effect on dampening incentives, suggesting that muscarinic receptors underlie the motivational influence on this preparatory activity. Taken together, these findings show that a muscarinic blocker impairs motivated action in healthy people, and that medial frontal preparatory neural activity mediates this for RT.
-
- Medicine
- Neuroscience
The advent of midazolam holds profound implications for modern clinical practice. The hypnotic and sedative effects of midazolam afford it broad clinical applicability. However, the specific mechanisms underlying the modulation of altered consciousness by midazolam remain elusive. Herein, using pharmacology, optogenetics, chemogenetics, fiber photometry, and gene knockdown, this in vivo research revealed the role of locus coeruleus (LC)-ventrolateral preoptic nucleus noradrenergic neural circuit in regulating midazolam-induced altered consciousness. This effect was mediated by α1 adrenergic receptors. Moreover, gamma-aminobutyric acid receptor type A (GABAA-R) represents a mechanistically crucial binding site in the LC for midazolam. These findings will provide novel insights into the neural circuit mechanisms underlying the recovery of consciousness after midazolam administration and will help guide the timing of clinical dosing and propose effective intervention targets for timely recovery from midazolam-induced loss of consciousness.