Intrinsic excitability mechanisms of neuronal ensemble formation
Abstract
Neuronal ensembles are coactive groups of cortical neurons, found in spontaneous and evoked activity, that can mediate perception and behavior. To understand the mechanisms that lead to the formation of ensembles, we co-activated layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons in brain slices from mouse visual cortex, in animals of both sexes, replicating in vitro an optogenetic protocol to generate ensembles in vivo. Using whole-cell and perforated patch-clamp pair recordings we found that, after optogenetic or electrical stimulation, coactivated neurons increased their correlated activity, a hallmark of ensemble formation. Coactivated neurons showed small biphasic changes in presynaptic plasticity, with an initial depression followed by a potentiation after a recovery period. Optogenetic and electrical stimulation also induced significant increases in frequency and amplitude of spontaneous EPSPs, even after single-cell stimulation. In addition, we observed unexpected strong and persistent increases in neuronal excitability after stimulation, with increases in membrane resistance and reductions in spike threshold. A pharmacological agent that blocks changes in membrane resistance reverted this effect. These significant increases in excitability can explain the observed biphasic synaptic plasticity. We conclude that cell-intrinsic changes in excitability are involved in the formation of neuronal ensembles. We propose an ‘iceberg’ model, by which increased neuronal excitability makes subthreshold connections suprathreshold, enhancing the effect of already existing synapses, and generating a new neuronal ensemble.
Editor's evaluation
This paper provides new insights regarding how the intrinsic excitability of neurons contributes to the formation of cortical ensembles, which may underlie memory formation. Previous work had left the relative contribution of intrinsic versus synaptic changes to neural ensemble formation incompletely understood. By using an interdisciplinary approach, the authors reveal the mechanisms by which changes in intrinsic excitability may play a unique role in the formation of new neural ensembles.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.77470.sa0eLife digest
In the brain, groups of neurons that are activated together – also known as neuronal ensembles – are the basic units that underpin perception and behavior. Yet, exactly how these coactive circuits are established remains under investigation.
In 1949, Canadian psychologist Donald Hebb proposed that, when brains learn something new, the neurons which are activated together connect to form ensembles, and their connections become stronger each time this specific piece of knowledge is recalled. This idea that ‘neurons that fire together, wire together’ can explain how memories are acquired and recalled, by strengthening their wiring.
However, recent studies have questioned whether strengthening connections is the only mechanism by which neural ensembles can be created. Changes in the excitability of neurons (how easily they are to fire and become activated) may also play a role. In other words, ensembles could emerge because certain neurons become more excitable and fire more readily.
To solve this conundrum, Alejandre-García et al. examined both hypotheses in the same system. Neurons in slices of the mouse visual cortex were stimulated electrically or optically, via a technique that controls neural activity with light. The activity of individual neurons and their connections was then measured with electrodes.
Spontaneous activity among connected neurons increased after stimulation, indicative of the formation of neuronal ensembles. Connected neurons also showed small changes in the strength of their connections, which first decreased and then rebounded after an initial recovery period.
Intriguingly, cells also showed unexpected strong and persistent increases in neuronal excitability after stimulation, such that neurons fired more readily to the same stimulus. In other words, neurons maintained a cellular memory of having been stimulated. The authors conclude that ensembles form because connected neurons become more excitable, which in turn, may strengthen connections of the circuit at a later stage.
These results provide fresh insights about the neural circuits underpinning learning and memory. In time, the findings could also help to understand disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease and schizophrenia, which are characterised by memory impairments and disordered thinking.
Introduction
The function of the brain is anchored on the activity of groups of connected neurons, forming microcircuits (Schüz and Braitenberg, 2001; Shepherd, 2004). In such circuits, groups of coactive neurons, or ensembles (also known as assemblies, synfire chains, or attractors), found in both spontaneous and evoked activity, exhibit synchronous or correlated activity (Abeles, 1991; Buzsáki, 2010; Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016; Cossart et al., 2003; Morris, 1999; Hopfield, 1982; Lorente De Nó, 1938; Marshel et al., 2019; Miller et al., 2014; Yuste, 2015) and could mediate memory formation (Morris, 1999), and functional brain states (Hoshiba et al., 2017). In the cerebral cortex, ensembles could arise from reverberating patterns of multineural activity, generated by recurrent connectivity, and occur spontaneously (Churchland and Sejnowski, 1992; Lorente De Nó, 1933). In mouse visual cortex, cortical ensembles are causally related to memory storage and percepts (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Marshel et al., 2019; Miller et al., 2014). Moreover, disorganized neuronal ensembles are observed in mouse models of schizophrenia (Hamm et al., 2017).
A hypothesis of how a neuronal ensemble (i.e. a neuronal assembly) could be formed was initially proposed by Hebb in 1949. Ensembles could be formed by synaptic plasticity among coactive neurons, as a consequence of the Hebbian rule, whereby the “persistent and repeated activation of connected neurons induce metabolic changes or growth of processes in one or both cells that strengthen those connections”. One synaptic mechanism that fulfills Hebb’s rule is long-term potentiation (LTP), a long-lasting increase in synaptic efficacy after a high frequency burst electrical stimulation, which is postulated to be the cellular correlate of learning (Bliss and Gardner-Medwin, 1973). With LTP, increased synaptic weights enhances preferential connectivity that leads to an increased recurrent activity within ensembles (Hoshiba et al., 2017). Consistent with this, synchronous stimulation of groups of neurons 50–100 times in vivo, using two-photon optogenetics, bound them together into an ‘imprinted’ ensemble that became spontaneously coactive after the optogenetic imprinting (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016). However, no direct evidence has yet demonstrated that the formation of ensembles depends on changes in synaptic connections.
As an alternative hypothesis, recent experiments have revealed widespread activity-dependent mechanisms in cortical neurons, suggesting that the formation and stability of an ensemble, or a memory engram, could be due to cell-autonomous intrinsic mechanisms, such as changes in neuronal excitability (Debanne and Poo, 2010; Gallistel and Balsam, 2014; Hansel and Disterhoft, 2020; Lisman et al., 2018; Titley et al., 2017). This hypothesis is consistent with experiments where specific patterns of activity in the hippocampus recalls stored memories, under protein synthesis inhibitors, which should prevent LTP (Pignatelli et al., 2019; Ryan et al., 2015; Tonegawa et al., 2015). Moreover, individual Purkinje cells can acquire and represent temporal pattern of inputs in a cell intrinsic manner, without synaptic plasticity (Johansson et al., 2014). Thus, ensemble formation could be due to intrinsic changes in the excitability state of the neurons, instead of, or in addition to, strengthening of its synapses (Disterhoft and Oh, 2006; Ganguly et al., 2000; Pignatelli et al., 2019).
To explore mechanisms of ensemble formation, we optogenetic and electrically stimulated neurons in mouse neocortical slices, replicating in vitro the optogenetic protocol used to imprint ensembles in vivo (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016). We then used whole-cell and perforated patch recordings, from individual neurons and connected pairs, to electrophysiologically characterize the stimulated neurons. We find moderate biphasic changes in synaptic strength, in the midst of major and generalized increases in cell-intrinsic excitability, that can, at least partly, explain the synaptic changes observed. We conclude that changes in excitability play a major role in ensemble formation.
Results
Spontaneous activity becomes correlated in simultaneously stimulated neurons
A neuronal ensemble can be defined as a group of neurons with correlated spontaneous and evoked activity (Buzsáki, 2010; Cossart et al., 2003; Ikegaya et al., 2004; Sasaki et al., 2007; Shepherd and Grillner, 2010; Stringer et al., 2019; Yuste, 2015). Previous results have demonstrated that optogenetic coactivation of neurons can generate new ensembles in vivo (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016). To explore the cellular and circuit mechanisms underlying this phenomenon, we studied the electrophysiological mechanisms that led to building of imprinted ensembles, replicating in vitro with the activation protocols that generates ensembles in vivo, with optogenetic or electrical stimulation. Experiments were performed in layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons expressing the opsin ChroME in slices from primary visual cortex of adult mice of both sexes (Figure 1A).
Optogenetic imprinting in vivo increased the correlation of spontaneous activity of stimulated neurons, indicative of the formation of a new ensemble (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016). To explore if our in vitro stimulation protocols were effective in generating ensembles, we used paired recordings to explore whether simultaneous and repeated optogenetic or electrical activation neurons also induced changes in the correlation of spontaneous activity. We recorded pairs of closely located pyramidal neurons and found that 25% of recorded pairs, at distances of 20–30 µm, were monosynaptically connected (7/28 pairs), consistent with previous reports (Cossell et al., 2015; Holmgren et al., 2003; Ko et al., 2011; Lefort et al., 2009; Levy and Reyes, 2012; Markram et al., 1997; Song et al., 2005; Figure 1A–C). Also, in agreement with previous studies, we found higher values of correlation of spontaneous activity between connected neuronal pairs (0.31±0.16; n=7) than between unconnected neuronal pairs (–0.04±0.37; n=21; p=0.049 by Mann-Whitney test, Figure 1F; Cossell et al., 2015; Ko et al., 2011; Yoshimura et al., 2005). However, ~50% (10/21) of the unconnected neuronal pairs still had relatively high correlated activity, comparable to connected neuronal pairs (0.32±0.18; n=9; all unconnected pairs with correlation >zero; Figure 1B). Notably, after optogenetic or electrical stimulation (Figure 1D and E), the correlation of spontaneous activity between neurons increased significantly, in both connected and unconnected pairs (0.02±0.06–0.2±0.05; n=13 pairs, 2 connected, 11 unconnected; p=0.049 by Wilcoxon; Figure 1G). No significant differences in correlations were found in unstimulated pairs (0.05±0.1–0.03±0.1, n=11 pairs, p=0.7 Wilcoxon, Figure 1H). We conclude that optogenetic and electrical stimulation can increase the correlation of spontaneous activity among neurons, a hallmark of neuronal ensemble formation.
Effect of optogenetic stimulation on monosynaptic currents
The creation of ensembles in vivo after optogenetic stimulation (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016), suggested that ensembles could be built by Hebbian synaptic plasticity, by potentiation of existing synapses of coactivated neurons. To explore this, we performed dual whole-cell and perforated patch-clamp recordings of connected neurons, and examined whether persistent and repeated optogenetic or electrical activation induced synaptic plasticity (Cossell et al., 2015; Morris, 1999). Action potentials were induced in the presynaptic neuron with depolarizing currents (10 pulses; 400–600 pA, 5ms, 20 Hz trains). For measuring synaptic currents, postsynaptic neurons were recorded in voltage-clamp, without current injection. We classified connections as monosynaptic if EPSC latency was 2±1 ms, relative to the presynaptic spike (Figure 2A; n=7). To normalize EPSC amplitudes, postsynaptic current values in the action potential start time were considered baseline. Current values for the first EPSC (EPSC1) during the stimulation trains, including failures, were averaged. A connection was defined as existent if the average evoked EPSC1 had an amplitude larger than 1.5 S.D. of the noise. Control recordings were performed with unconnected neurons (n=21 pairs), and the optogenetic protocol did no generate any new detectable synaptic connections.
-
Figure 2—source data 1
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/77470/elife-77470-fig2-data1-v2.xlsx
-
Figure 2—source data 2
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/77470/elife-77470-fig2-data2-v2.xlsx
-
Figure 2—source data 3
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/77470/elife-77470-fig2-data3-v2.xlsx
In pairs of connected neurons, we examined if optogenetic stimulation induced changes in monosynaptic currents, pooling data from whole-cell (n=5) and perforated patch-clamp (n=2) recordings. The average amplitude of monosynaptic ESPCs in control condition agreed with previous reports (Feldmeyer et al., 2006; Morishima et al., 2011; Sempere-Ferràndez et al., 2019; Yoshimura et al., 2005). Although average EPSC1 peak currents of optogenetically stimulated neurons did not show significant changes with stimulation, they revealed a trend towards amplitude depression after the stimulation, followed with a tendency towards potentiation, after a 20 min rest period (before: 13±7 pA; after: 9±7 pA; p=0.2 Wilcoxon; n=7, Figure 2B and C). Control EPSC1 amplitudes of unstimulated neurons did not show significant difference before (13±2 pA) and after 30 min of recordings (12±2 pA; p=0.1 Wilcoxon; n=11, whole-cell recordings, n=7; perforated patch-clamp, n=4). To evaluate changes in short-term plasticity, we calculated the paired-pulse ratio (PPR), that is the ratio of amplitudes of the second to the first EPSC in the train (EPSC2/EPSC1). We integrated data from whole-cell (n=5) and perforated patch-clamp recordings (n=2), and found that PPR did not significantly change with the stimulation (before: 0.95±0.09 PPR; after: 0.7±0.1 PPR; p=0.4 Wilcoxon; n=7; Figure 2D). Similarly, control unstimulated neurons in whole-cell (n=4) and perforated patch-clamp recordings (n=7) did not show significant changes in PPR before (1.08±0.12) and after 30 min (1. 06±0.12; p=0.7 Wilcoxon; n=11).
We concluded that optogenetic stimulation did not have a significant effect on monosynaptic currents, although with a consistent trend towards an initial depression, followed with a subsequent potentiation.
Biphasic plasticity of monosynaptic currents after electrical stimulation
We carried out similar experiments with electrical stimulation of connected pairs of neurons, thus avoiding possible side effects caused by opsin expression (see Materials and methods) or the optogenetic stimulation of other neurons expressing opsin in the field of view. Using perforated patch-clamp, we recorded and electrically stimulated pairs of connected pyramidal neurons. After electrical stimulation, EPSC1 amplitudes decreased significantly (from 14±2 pA to 8±1 pA; p=0.016 by Wilcoxon; n=7; Figure 2E). After pausing the stimulation for 20 min, the amplitude of evoked ESPCs recovered, with a moderate potentiation. Specifically, for electrically stimulated neurons, EPSC1 amplitude recovered significantly from 8±1–20±1 pA after 20 min of post-stimulation rest (p=0.015 by Wilcoxon; n=7). The amplitude after the recovery period was moderately larger than the EPSCs amplitude before stimulation (14±2–20±1 pA; p=0.015 by Wilcoxon; Figure 2E). In contrast, control unstimulated neurons did not show a significant change (before: 13±2 pA; after 30 min: 12±2 pA; p=0.6 by Wilcoxon; n=8). We also measured the PPR of electrical stimulated neurons before and after electrical stimulation, finding no significant differences (before: 1.3±0.15 PPR; after: 1.03±0.18 PPR; p=0.7 Wilcoxon; n=7). No difference in PPR was observed before and after 20 min post-stimulation rest (1.3±0.13; p=0.1 by Wilcoxon; Figure 2E). Similarly, the PPR of unstimulated neurons did not change before (1.4±0.1) and after 30 min without stimulation (1.3±0.15; p=0.5 by Wilcoxon; n=7) (Figure 2F).
We conclude that electrical coactivation leads to a moderate biphasic change in monosynaptic currents, with an initial depression followed by a potentiation. These results are consistent with the trends found in pairs of optogenetic stimulated neurons, where EPSC1 amplitude after a post-stimulation recovery period increased from 9±7 pA to 19±3 pA, although no statistical comparison was possible due to the low n (Figure 2C). The difficulty in obtaining perforated whole-cell recordings from connected pairs of neurons in opsin expressing cells from adult brain slices precluded us from increasing the n after many attempts. These difficulties were ameliorated in the electrical stimulation experiments.
Biphasic plasticity of synaptic potentials after stimulation
To further explore potential postsynaptic mechanisms of ensemble formation, in parallel experiments, we used current-clamp to examine postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) in connected pairs, comparing evoked EPSP amplitudes before and after optogenetic and electrical stimulation (Figure 2G). We combined data from optogenetically (n=2, blue) and electrically-stimulated pairs (n=7, gray). In good agreement with the voltage-clamp data, the average peak amplitude of the first EPSP (EPSP1) significantly decreased after stimulation (0.43±0.2 mV to 0.28±0.2 mV; p=0.004 by Wilcoxon; n=9; Figure 2H). As with EPSC measurements, EPSPs recovered and became potentiated after 20 min post-stimulation to 0.8±0.2 mV (recovery compared with before: p=0.008; and after stimulation: p=0.004 by Wilcoxon). Control unstimulated neurons did not show significant differences in EPSP amplitude before (0.44±0.2 mV) or after 30 min (0.42±0.2 mV; p=0.7 by Wilcoxon; n=10).
In conclusion, paired recordings, in both current and voltage clamp, revealed that the coactivation of neurons generates a moderate biphasic synaptic plasticity, with an initial small depression, followed by a rebound potentiation, after several minutes of rest.
Biphasic synaptic plasticity in local circuit after optogenetic stimulation
To explore synaptic plasticity in the local circuit of optogenetically stimulated neurons, we also performed voltage-clamp and current-clamp recordings of evoked EPSCs and EPSPs in neurons without opsin expression, but located in the same field of view (Figure 3A). Assuming an area photostimulated by the LED of 150 µM in radius, we estimated that the number of superficial neurons with opsin expression potentially activated by the light was ~20 cells. Thus, optical stimulation could lead to an increase in the activity of the local circuit. To monitor this, we used perforated patch-clamp from non-expressing pyramidal neurons, and measured EPSCs or EPSPs evoked by the LED, in both voltage and current-clamp. Light-evoked EPSCs or EPSPs were analyzed if the latency was >2ms relative to LED pulse onset (Figure 3A; n=7). In non-expressing cells, population light-evoked EPSC1 (22±3 pA) and EPSP1 amplitudes (2±0.1 mV) were significantly higher than unitary evoked EPSC1 (14±2 pA; p=0.04 by Mann-Whitney) or EPSP1 (0.43±0.2 mV; p=0.0006 by Mann-Whitney) found in connected pairs. This is likely because LED stimulated several neurons in the field simultaneously, generating a compound EPSC and EPSP.
After optogenetic stimulation (10 Hz of 5ms pulse for 4 s every 10 s; 30 min), the average light-evoked EPSC1 showed a significant diminution in amplitude (22±3–11±1 pA; p=0.015 by Wilcoxon; n=7; Figure 3B), and a recovery with potentiation after 20 min post-stimulation (40±3 pA; p=0.015, comparing with before; p=0.015, Wilcoxon). Similarly, light-evoked EPSPs showed a significant decrease after 30 min optogenetic stimulation (2±0.1 mV to 1.3±0.2 mV; p=0.015 by Wilcoxon; n=7; Figure 3C), and also recovered with a moderate increase in amplitude after 20 min post-stimulation (2.5±0.3 mV; p=0.015, comparing with before; p=0.015, Wilcoxon). These results, in excellent agreement with the connected pair data, reveals that the biphasic synaptic plasticity is also found in unstimulated neurons, thus suggesting that it is a circuit-wide phenomenon.
Increased frequency and amplitude of spontaneous synaptic inputs after optogenetic and electrical stimulation
To further explore the effect of the optogenetic and electrical stimulation on population activity, we measured spontaneous EPSPs from recorded neurons, and compared them before and after stimulation (Figure 4A). To measure spontaneous EPSPs, we low-pass filtered current-clamp recordings, subtracting membrane potential oscillations, to obtain comparable baselines of EPSPs amplitudes (Juárez-Vidales et al., 2021). We detected EPSPs with amplitudes between 0.3 mV and 10 mV and analyzed changes in the frequency of spontaneous EPSPs before and after optogenetic stimulation (Figure 4A–B). The frequency of spontaneous EPSPs significant increased from 90±9 EPSPs/min before to 127±19 after optogenetic stimulation (p=0.004 by Wilcoxon; n=10) (Figure 4D left). We also observed increases in EPSPs amplitude (0.38±0.01–0.41±0.02 mV; p=0.04) (Figure 4E left). In control recordings (Figure 4D–E right), non-expressing pyramidal neurons showed no statistically differences in the frequency of spontaneous EPSPs (93±25–64±18; p=0.06), or in EPSPs amplitude (0.37±0.004–0.37±0.004 mV; p=0.5; n=7).
Similarly, electrical stimulated neurons showed a significant increase in the frequency of spontaneous EPSPs from 80±12–118±17 EPSPs/min (p=0.001 by Wilcoxon; n=13) and EPSPs amplitude (0.37±0.03–0.40±0.03 mV; p=0.03) (Figure 3F–G left). In unstimulated neurons (Figure 3F–G right), we found no statistically difference in the frequency of spontaneous EPSPs (98±18–93±15; p=0.4), or any significant differences in EPSPs amplitude (0.37±0.01–0.38±0.01 mV; p=0.4; n=12).
These results reveal significant changes in spontaneous synaptic inputs after optogenetic and electrical stimulation. The increase in spontaneous EPSPs amplitude could partly explain their increased frequencies, as smaller EPSPs will become detectable. Importantly, since increases in spontaneous EPSPs frequency and amplitude also occur after electrical stimulation of individual neurons, our results demonstrate that neurons become more sensitive to synaptic inputs after stimulation, in a cell intrinsic manner.
Optogenetic stimulation increases neuronal excitability
Our results demonstrated that optogenetic and electrical stimulation, even of individual neurons, could induce changes in intrinsic cellular excitability. This is in agreement with recent literature that has described non-synaptic cellular plasticity in a variety of experimental preparations (Abraham et al., 2019; Campanac et al., 2008; Ganguly et al., 2000; Malik and Chattarji, 2012; Marder and Goaillard, 2006; Paz et al., 2009; Ryan et al., 2015; Titley et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2005; Yang and Santamaria, 2016). To explore these mechanisms, we evaluated active and passive intrinsic electrophysiological parameters, such as current injection-dependent firing, frequency, membrane resistance, membrane potential, and firing threshold. We recorded individual neurons with perforated patch-clamp configuration before and after optogenetic and electrical stimulation (Figure 5).
We first measured membrane potential dependency to current injections with I-V plots in pyramidal neurons with opsin expression. To do this, we kept the membrane potential at –70 mV and applied series of 500ms current injections ranging from –100 pA to 160 pA, with 20 pA alternating positive and negative steps (black traces; Figure 5A). We first observed a significant increase in evoked number of spikes after optogenetic stimulation (blue traces; 5±1–6±1 spikes; p=0.015 by Wilcoxon; n=12; Figure 5D–a). These increases in firing rate were not observed in unstimulated neurons (5±1 and 5±1; p=1 by Wilcoxon; n=7; Figure 5D–b). We also noticed that neurons expressing opsin exhibited a small alteration in their intrinsic properties. In particular, the firing rate became higher than in neurons without opsin in control condition (5±1 firing rate of neurons with opsin expression, 3.5±1 neurons without opsin, but this difference was not significant p=0.1 by Mann-Whitney; n=7).
Neurons needed at least 15–20 min of optogenetic or electrical stimulation to increase their excitability, but, after their firing rate increased, it was stable for the 80 min duration of the experiment (6±1–6±1 spikes; p=0.5 by Wilcoxon; n=7; Figure 5B). The long-lasting increase in firing rate did not require continued stimulation, since after pausing optogenetic stimulation for 30 min, it remained elevated (6±1–6±1 spikes; p=0.8 by Wilcoxon; n=7; Figure 5C). Similar results were found for the average instantaneous firing frequency, which increased after optogenetic stimulation from 15±3–17±3 Hz; p=0.015 by Wilcoxon; n=12; Figure 5E–a. Unstimulated neurons did not show significant differences (from 16±3–16±3 Hz; p=0.5 by Wilcoxon; n=7; Figure 5E–b). Finally, to evaluate the modification of firing properties that could facilitate burst induction, we measured the mean frequency of the two first spikes for each current step, finding a significant increase (before: 24±3 Hz; after optogenetic stimulation: 27±3 Hz; p=0.015 Wilcoxon; Figure 5F–a), whereas unstimulated neurons did not show significant differences after 30 minutes of recordings (25±6 and 24±6 Hz; p=0.2 by Wilcoxon; n=7; Figure 5F–b). To rule out that these changes in firing properties were due to the illumination, we recorded neurons without opsin expression in perforated patch-clamp, before and after optogenetic stimulation. We did not detect any significant change in current injection firing-dependence (4±1 and 4±1 spikes; p=1, by Wilcoxon; n=7), nor in the average instantaneous frequency (7±2 Hz before; and 7±2 Hz after; p=0.6 by Wilcoxon). These data, similar to those from unstimulated neurons, demonstrate that the LED illumination does not induce changes in cellular excitability.
Terfenadine reverts optogenetic stimulation increases neuronal excitability
These results revealed that optogenetic stimulation produced a robust increase in current-dependent firing. To pharmacologically explore potential mechanisms underlying these changes, we used terfenadine, a pharmacological agent that blocks currents mediated by Ether-a-go-go Related Gene (ERG) channels, and, in neocortical pyramidal cells, prevents persistent firing and increases in membrane resistance (Cui and Strowbridge, 2018). Indeed, the increases in firing rate and frequencies induced by optogenetic stimulation reverted to control conditions after application of 10 µM terfenadine (5±1 and 4±1 spikes; p=0.015 by Wilcoxon; n=7; Figure 5D–c). No differences were found comparing with before stimulation and when terfenadine was applied (4±1 and 4±1 spikes; p=0.06 by Wilcoxon; n=7). In unstimulated neurons without opsin, terfenadine generated a reduction in firing rate, consistent with a baseline activation of terfenadine-sensitive conductances (4±1 before; and 3±1 terfenadine p=0.03 by Wilcoxon; n=7). We concluded the increase in excitability due to optogenetic stimulation can be reverted by terfenadine. This could be due to blockage of baseline conductances, such as ERG channels or other targets like histamine1, kir 6, and Kv11.1 (Carter et al., 1985).
Electrical stimulation increases neuronal excitability
After characterizing the changes in firing properties due to optogenetic activation, we explored if single-cell electrical stimulation also generated similar alterations. Electrical stimulation via a patch pipette avoids possible side effects produced by the optogenetic stimulation, or by the activation of an undetermined number of opsin-expressing neurons. Electrical stimulation of one neuron at the time also rules out synaptic effects. In these single-cell electrical stimulation experiments, we observed similar effects as in optogenetic stimulation: increases in firing rate (before 4±1 spikes; after 5±1 spikes; p=0.015 by Wilcoxon; n=22; Figure 5D–d); increases in average frequency (8±2 Hz before; 10±2 Hz after; p=0.015 by Wilcoxon; Figure 5E–d); and increases in frequency of the first two spikes (14±3 Hz before; 17±3 Hz after; p=0.015 by Wilcoxon; Figure 5F–d). Unstimulated neurons did not show any changes in firing rate (before 4±1; after 4±1 spikes; p=1 by Wilcoxon; n=10; Figure 5D–e); or average frequency (9±2 Hz before; and 9±2 Hz after; p=1 by Wilcoxon; Figure 5E–e); nor in the first frequency (15±3 Hz before; and 15±3 Hz after; p=1 by Wilcoxon; Figure 5F–e).
The consistency in results from optogenetic and electrical stimulation indicate that the increases in cellular excitability are not due to opsin expression or optogenetic protocol, and is a property of individual neurons that can be induced by stimulation of individual cells.
Optogenetic and electrical stimulation increase membrane resistance
An increase in excitability could be due to an increase in membrane resistance (Marder and Goaillard, 2006). To calculate membrane resistance, we measured membrane potential changes produced by hyperpolarizing current pulses of low intensity (from 40 to –100 pA; 500ms duration, applied every second), before and after optogenetic stimulation (Figure 6A). Membrane input resistance was then measured as the slope of the voltage-current (I-V) relationship (Figure 6B). We observed a significant increase in input resistance from 278±18 mΩ to 305±22 mΩ after optogenetic stimulation (p=0.0001 by Wilcoxon; n=14; Figure 6C–a). Meanwhile, resistances of unstimulated neurons, with opsin expression, did not show any change before (318±23 mΩ) or 30 min after stimulation (309±24 mΩ; p=0.9 by Wilcoxon; n=8; Figure 6C–b). These increases were reverted by the addition of terfenadine (from 172±15 mΩ after stimulation to 158±16 mΩ after terfenadine; p=0.02 by Wilcoxon; n=9; Figure 6C–c). Also, addition of terfenadine brought back the resistance to its pre-stimulation values, 158±15 mΩ before stimulation to 153±16 mΩ with terfenadine; p=0.9 by Wilcoxon. Furthermore, electrical stimulation in non-expressing neurons also induced a significant increase in input resistance (from 176±9–185±9 mΩ; p=0.0003 by Wilcoxon; n=11; Figure 6C–d). Unstimulated neurons did not display changes in membrane resistance (before 171±11 to after 174±8 mΩ; p=0.3 by Wilcoxon; n=11; Figure 6C–e). Control experiment with non-expressing neurons did not show any alteration in membrane resistance after optogenetic stimulation of the slices (before 176±20 mΩ; after 179±20 mΩ; p=0.3 by Wilcoxon).
-
Figure 6—source data 1
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/77470/elife-77470-fig6-data1-v2.xlsx
-
Figure 6—source data 2
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/77470/elife-77470-fig6-data2-v2.xlsx
-
Figure 6—source data 3
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/77470/elife-77470-fig6-data3-v2.xlsx
It has been reported that membrane resting potential can also change through increases in excitability, resulting in a depolarization (Debanne et al., 2019; Mellor et al., 2002; Ross and Soltesz, 2001). To explore this, we calculated membrane resting potential as the median of spontaneous membrane activity without any current injection. We observed that resting potential remained stable under all condition: before and after optogenetic stimulation (–73±2 to -72±1 mV; p=0.4 by Wilcoxon; n=10; Figure 6D–a); unstimulated neurons (–73±2 to -75±2 mV; p=0.8 by Wilcoxon; n=13; Figure 6D–b). The application of terfenadine after stimulation also did not reveal significant changes in membrane potential (–78±2 to -77±2 mV; p=0.8 by Wilcoxon; n=8; Figure 6D–c) and no changes before and after electrical stimulation (–80±1 to -79±1 mV; p=0.9 by Wilcoxon; n=13; Figure 6D–d). This lack of resting membrane potential changes during intrinsic neuronal excitability agrees with previous reports (Cudmore and Turrigiano, 2004; Disterhoft et al., 1986; Pignatelli et al., 2019). We conclude that the increase in excitability induced by optogenetic or electrical stimulation is associated with increases in membrane resistance, but not with membrane resting potential.
Lowering of action potential threshold after optogenetic and electrical stimulation
The changes in frequencies of the first two spikes that we observed could also be due to spike threshold changes. Burst firing depends on the interplay between the afterhyperpolarization (AHPs) and afterdepolarizations (ADPs) that follow the first action potential (Brumberg et al., 2000; Mahon and Charpier, 2012; Paz et al., 2009). To explore this, we evaluated the dynamics of action potentials with phase plot analysis. We used the first spikes from 60 pA current injections in the I-V dataset; because during this current step, most neurons consistently displayed action potentials before and after optogenetic or electrical stimulation protocol. Action potentials were detected after setting a threshold of >0.00015 in the first derivative of the membrane potential waveform, a value that corresponded to the firing threshold, and voltage/time values (mV/ms) were plotted as a function of membrane potential (mV). In this analysis, action potentials are represented as a loop in which the starting point represents the firing threshold (Figure 7A). After optogenetic stimulation, the majority of phase plots displayed a leftward shift, making evident a lowering in firing threshold (–41±1 before to –43.7±1 mV after; p=0.0012 by Wilcoxon; n=13; Figure 7B–a). This change was partially reverted by terfenadine (10 µM; –45±1 mV after stimulation to –44±1 mV after terfenadine; p=0.05 by Wilcoxon; n=8; Figure 7B–c). We found no statistically significant differences in firing threshold of unstimulated neurons, when comparing the beginning and the end of the recording (–41±2 and –41±2 mV respectively; p=0.7 by Wilcoxon; n=8 cells; Figure 7B–b). Also, non-expressing neurons did not show any alteration of firing threshold after optogenetic stimulation of the slices (before –40±1.4 mV to –40±1.5 mV after; p=0.7 by Wilcoxon). Thus, the LED light did not induce damage or alterations in this cell-intrinsic parameter.
We also documented a similar lowering of the threshold voltage in electrically stimulated neurons (before –42±0.8 mV; after –44±0.8 mV; p=0.00004 by Wilcoxon; n=22 cells; Figure 7B–d). Unstimulated neurons displayed no changes in threshold voltage (–42±0.7 mV at the first min and –43±0.8 mV 30 min later; p=0.5 by Wilcoxon; n=11; Figure 7B–e).
Combined increase in membrane resistance and reduction of firing threshold, can explain the increase in neuronal excitability previously observed. Thus, optogenetic and electrical stimulation significantly increases action potential firing in response to synaptic inputs, facilitating the formation of a neuronal ensemble.
Discussion
Since Hebb, the traditional view of how a neuronal ensemble (i.e., an assembly) is created by strengthening its synaptic connections after repeated coactivation (Bliss and Gardner-Medwin, 1973; Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016; Cossell et al., 2015; Morris, 1999; Hoshiba et al., 2017). However, several reports have questioned whether synaptic plasticity is the only mechanism for circuit plasticity, proposing instead cell-intrinsic mechanisms (Angelo et al., 2012; Brown et al., 2019; Disterhoft and Oh, 2006; Ganguly et al., 2000; Marder and Goaillard, 2006; Ryan et al., 2015; Zhang and Linden, 2003). Consistent with this, an in contradiction with our own predictions that synaptic plasticity mediated neuronal ensembles (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016), we describe here that simultaneous optogenetic or electrical stimulation of neurons in vitro increases the excitability of the stimulated cells. The increases in correlation among neurons, generated by an increased excitability, could also contribute to generate or enhance long-term synaptic plasticity (Lisman et al., 2018; Titley et al., 2017). Together, cellular and synaptic mechanisms could contribute to neuronal ensemble development (Penn and Shatz, 1999; Zhang and Poo, 2001), flexibility, plasticity (Clopath et al., 2017; Kirkwood et al., 1996; Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2007; Pérez-Ortega et al., 2021; Wandell and Smirnakis, 2009; Wiesel and Hubel, 1963).
Role of synaptic mechanisms in ensemble formation
Our experimental procedures, similar to those that build (‘imprint’) neuronal ensembles in vivo (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016), lead to increases in correlations of spontaneous activity, a hallmark of a neuronal ensemble, just as it does in vivo. Using paired recordings, we find similar rates of connectivity between neuronal pairs as previous reports (Cossell et al., 2015; Holmgren et al., 2003; Ko et al., 2011; Lefort et al., 2009; Levy and Reyes, 2012; Markram et al., 1997; Song et al., 2005). Also, consistent with many reports, connected neurons display higher correlation in spontaneous activity than unconnected ones (Cossell et al., 2015; Ko et al., 2011; Yoshimura et al., 2005). Interestingly, our results also showed that 50% of unconnected neurons can be as correlated as connected neurons, and contrary to our own expectations, we did not detect any new connections formed after optogenetic stimulation in previously unconnected neurons.
Based on previous studies, there is preferential connectivity between neurons with similar receptive fields (Cossell et al., 2015; Ko et al., 2011; Yoshimura et al., 2005). Increased synaptic connectivity also probably underlies pattern completion in cortical ensembles (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Pignatelli et al., 2019), and a few strong connections could drive local excitation of the majority of neurons with weak connectivity (Cossell et al., 2015; Holmgren et al., 2003; Ko et al., 2011; Lefort et al., 2009; Levy and Reyes, 2012; Markram et al., 1997; Song et al., 2005; Figure 8). Consistent with these hypotheses, we expected to observe synaptic efficiency changes between connected neurons after co-stimulation (Bliss and Gardner-Medwin, 1973). But, rather than synaptic potentiation, our stimulation protocols induced a moderate biphasic plasticity of connected neurons and also in the local circuit, with an initial depression followed by a recovery potentiation of evoked EPSCs and EPSPs after 20 min of rest.
This result agrees with reports that EPSP amplitude can decrease after high stimulation frequencies, but recovers from depression by using lower stimulation rates and, after a period of rest can rebound which is revealed as a potentiation (Feldmeyer et al., 2006). We cannot rule out that Hebbian synaptic plasticity contributes to this potentiation. However, an alternative explanation is that this could instead be due to a depletion, followed by a recovery, of the ready releasable pool of synaptic vesicles in the presynaptic neuron (Feldmeyer et al., 2006; Schneggenburger et al., 2002).
In addition to these synaptic changes, our results demonstrate that persistent and simultaneous stimulation robustly increases membrane resistance. Thus, neuronal excitability could increase the efficiency of existing synaptic connections between neurons in an ensemble, without significant synaptic plasticity. Indeed, we find that spontaneous EPSPs become amplified in single optogenetic and electrically stimulated neurons. A larger synaptic input could also naturally result from an overall increase in neuronal firing too, since increased neuronal excitability would result in increase in neurotransmitter release or amplified EPSPs. This agrees with reports that reveal that synaptic inputs can be enhanced by intrinsic membrane mechanisms (Campanac and Debanne, 2008; Hu et al., 2002; Li et al., 2020; Manabe et al., 1992; Marder and Goaillard, 2006; Narayanan and Johnston, 2008; Turrigiano et al., 1998). For all these reasons, we suspect the modest synaptic plasticity that we observe can be explained by the effects of the stimulation protocols in the ready releasable pool and intrinsic changes in excitability in the stimulated neurons and the network.
Primary visual cortex (V1) responds to sensory information. Thus, one of the main roles of V1 neurons is to integrate sensory inputs (Buonomano and Merzenich, 1998; Mountcastle, 1997; Figure 8). Unitary EPSP, that is, pyramidal to pyramidal monosynaptic connections, would not be expected to increase the firing probability of postsynaptic neurons, in vitro or in vivo (Holmgren et al., 2003; Jouhanneau et al., 2018). However, the joint activation of several neurons in the local circuit (in our case: neighbor neurons with opsin expression), could evoke compound EPSC and EPSP with amplitudes larger than unitary EPSC or EPSP, and could generate synfire chains of activity that propagate through the cortex (Abeles, 1991). Thus, increases in synaptic efficacy, mediated by an increase in neuronal excitability of connected neurons, could play a significant role in the local circuit, improving the efficiency of the flow of the information during visual stimulation and during the recall of neurons associated with a memory.
Role of intrinsic mechanisms in ensemble formation
Changes in intrinsic properties are known to alter EPSP amplitude, for example, by the modification of membrane conductance induced by postsynaptic stimulation (Hu et al., 2002; Li et al., 2020; Manabe et al., 1992; Marder and Goaillard, 2006; Narayanan and Johnston, 2008; Turrigiano et al., 1998). These changes might be due to modification in intrinsic membrane conductances (Li et al., 2020; Manabe et al., 1992; Narayanan and Johnston, 2008), and they could amplify synaptic inputs and improve the accuracy of synaptic integration of neurons (Desai et al., 1999; Mahon and Charpier, 2012; Malik and Chattarji, 2012; Sjöström et al., 2008; Turrigiano et al., 1994).
Confirming the importance of intrinsic mechanisms, in our experiments, robust increases in firing rate were easily detected in optogenetic and electrically stimulated neurons. Even though we noticed that the expression of opsin by itself can have small effects in excitability, similar changes were observed in non-expressing neurons after electrical stimulation. This rules out that the changes in excitability observed were due to the opsin expression. An increase in cellular activity through cell-intrinsic mechanisms after prolonged firing agrees with many functional studies in vertebrates, using different protocols (Abraham et al., 2019; Ryan et al., 2015; Titley et al., 2017). In fact, increases in excitability are also found following LTP induction in visual cortical neurons (Cudmore and Turrigiano, 2004), in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells (Campanac and Debanne, 2008; Ganguly et al., 2000; Xu et al., 2005), and after behavior training in CA1 (Disterhoft et al., 1986). Intrinsic excitability is also modified when an animal is exposed to novel sensorial experience (Brown et al., 2019) or after environmental enrichment (Valero-Aracama et al., 2015).
The intensity-dependent and persistent increase in firing rate after stimulation protocol could be due to changes in membrane resistance and firing threshold. The synergy between these mechanisms could make neurons reach firing threshold with smaller depolarizations. Thus, an increase in excitability would be particularly relevant for lower current injections generated by weaker inputs. We propose that, after optogenetic or electrical stimulation, neurons shift to a more excitable state, like an iceberg emerging out of the water (Figure 8). Therefore, the same unchanged synaptic inputs could bring neurons to threshold and induce increased output firing after repetitive stimulation, improving the synaptic efficiency in pyramidal neurons (Debanne et al., 2019; Lisman et al., 2018; Nicoll et al., 1993; Titley et al., 2017; Zhang and Linden, 2003). Thus, neuronal optogenetic and electrical stimulation might alter the circuit without changing the synapses themselves, and this phenomenon could occur quickly.
Mechanisms of increased excitability in ensemble formation
Modifications in sub- and suprathreshold membrane conductances that initiate an action potential could underlie increased excitability (Brumberg et al., 2000). In terms of passive properties, our results coincide with many studies that have shown alterations in input resistance as the mechanism responsible for increases in excitability (Aizenman and Linden, 2000; Aou et al., 1992; Armano et al., 2000; Brown et al., 2019; Disterhoft et al., 1986; Marder and Goaillard, 2006; Woody et al., 1991; Xu et al., 2005). Activity-dependent increases in input resistance have been shown in hippocampal neurons. There, the increase in excitability could be generated by suppression of G-protein-coupled inwardly rectifying K+ channels (GIRK channels) (Valero-Aracama et al., 2015); or by cAMP-responsive element-binding protein (CREB)-dependent control of excitability by reducing K+ conductance. (Lisman et al., 2018; Yu et al., 2017) or potassium and sodium voltage gate conductances (Campanac and Debanne, 2008; Ganguly et al., 2000). Additionally, the contribution of H-channels in intrinsic membrane properties (Campanac et al., 2008; Fan et al., 2005). However, that voltage sag, which corresponds to current-H, is largely absent in layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons (Kalmbach et al., 2018; van Aerde and Feldmeyer, 2015). Our experiment showed only a few neurons that displayed a modest current-H, but it was not affected by optogenetic or electrical stimulation (not shown).
Moreover, recent studies show that persisting firing could be mediated by an Ether-à-gogo Related Gene (ERG) K+ channel in neocortical pyramidal neurons (Cui and Strowbridge, 2018; Debanne et al., 2019). In this view, calcium entry induced by repetitive neuronal action potential could modulate leak potassium currents in the neurons, which will generate persistent activity. Consistent with this, in our experiments, the blockage of ERG channels by terfenadine returned the firing rate and the increased input resistance induced by optogenetic and electrical stimulation to basal conditions. Thus, ERG channels appear to have a tonic activity in pyramidal layer 2/3 neurons.
The cell-intrinsic mechanisms that induce excitability are cell-type and also, stimulation protocol-dependent (Angelo et al., 2012). Hence, different changes in action potential firing have been identified. One of the most common changes is a reduction in after-hyperpolarization (AHP) amplitude, described in hippocampal neurons (Disterhoft et al., 1986; Disterhoft and Oh, 2006; Malik and Chattarji, 2012; Pignatelli et al., 2019; Zhang and Linden, 2003). In our experiments, we found a significant decrease in spike threshold as the most robust change in optogenetic and electrical stimulated neurons. This parameter also determines the firing frequency of neurons associated with voltage-dependent sodium or calcium currents. Changes in threshold appear to be usual in cortical neurons after intrinsic plasticity induction (Brumberg et al., 2000; Cudmore and Turrigiano, 2004; Mahon and Charpier, 2012; Paz et al., 2009), hippocampal neurons (Malik and Chattarji, 2012; Valero-Aracama et al., 2015; Xu et al., 2005), and cerebellar Purkinje cells (Aizenman and Linden, 2000; Armano et al., 2000). Consistent with this, visual deprivation increases spike threshold in pyramidal neurons of visual cortex, as an example of a mechanism that reduces neuronal excitability (Brown et al., 2019). Also, the persistent sodium current could underlie the lowering of spike threshold, throughout the modulation of protein kinase C (Astman et al., 1998; Ganguly et al., 2000; Valero-Aracama et al., 2015); or protein kinase A activation-dependent on calcium influx (Cudmore and Turrigiano, 2004). Finally, potassium channels Kv1 can regulate the action potential threshold (Feria Pliego and Pedroarena, 2020). Therefore, the intrinsic plasticity that we report could result from modifications of voltage-gated ion channels and inward rectification by potassium channels. Future studies will examine the exact molecular mechanisms that underlying these increases in excitability.
Contribution of intrinsic excitability and synaptic plasticity to circuit plasticity
The apparently contradictory evidence supporting Hebbian synaptic plasticity or intrinsic mechanisms of circuit modification and ensemble formation could be reconciled if one considers the experimental protocol used, as well as temporal and mechanistic factors. As a note of caution, we should mention that the stimulation protocols we used are artificial, and the imprinting of ensembles with optogenetics or electrical stimulation may not be representative of the natural ensembles found and created in the cortex under physiological conditions. Thus, the balance of mechanisms we find may differ with in vivo ensembles, under naturalistic plasticity conditions. In particular, the optogenetic imprinting model for ensemble formation we used occurs quickly in vivo, as soon as we can measure it, within a few minutes. We also see fast changes in neuronal correlations in vitro after optogenetic and electrical stimulation. Although these fast changes are explained by fast intrinsic modifications in excitability, imprinted ensembles could be different from the circuit modification that occur during learning, which, for visual tasks, can take several days or weeks in the same circuit we study (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019), and which could engage long-term plasticity, Hebbian or not Hebbian.
In addition, intrinsic plasticity can underlie or contribute to Hebbian plasticity by enhancing the probability for subsequent LTP induction and form or stabilize ensembles/engrams (Debanne et al., 2019; Lisman et al., 2018). Alternatively, intrinsic plasticity could add neurons into ensembles, even when synaptic weights do not change (Hansel and Disterhoft, 2020; Titley et al., 2017). A third possibility is that behavioral training could engage Hebbian mechanisms that would then induce increases in neuronal excitability (Disterhoft et al., 1986; Malik and Chattarji, 2012; Valero-Aracama et al., 2015). So, circuit plasticity could be due to the interplay of synaptic and intrinsic mechanisms and this balance could be different for short and long-term plasticity. In this view, ensembles could be quickly formed by fast intrinsic mechanisms but then be modified by slower synaptic plasticity, perhaps in relation with learning.
To conclude, our work demonstrates that significant and generalized increases in cellular excitability by intrinsic mechanisms are found during ensemble formation, induced by optogenetic and electrical stimulation. The possibility of modifying input and output patterns of neurons through changes in its firing properties could increase synaptic efficacy (Brumberg et al., 2000; Lisman, 1997) and the correlation between neurons, as we have demonstrated. This could provide an ideal setting for follow-up Hebbian synaptic plasticity than could occur more slowly. As a form of cellular memory, intrinsic excitability could also implement temporary stimulus-response mappings and might play a role in rapid cognitive flexibility (Pang and Fairhall, 2019). Faster ‘online’ plasticity via intrinsic excitability could have a broad impact on network dynamics and could serve as a critical information-storage mechanism that may contribute to memory formation (Lisman et al., 2018; Marder and Goaillard, 2006; Pérez-Ortega et al., 2021; Pignatelli et al., 2019; Xu et al., 2005) and to the generation of intrinsic circuit states, such as ensembles and circuit attractors (Buzsáki, 2010; Carrillo-Reid et al., 2019; Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016; Hopfield, 1982; Miller et al., 2014).
Materials and methods
All procedures were performed by following the U.S. National Institutes of Health and Columbia University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines (IACUC, Protocol #AC-AAAV3464). Experiments were carried out on C57BL/6 transgenic mice (Vglut1-Cre, Jackson Laboratories; RRID:IMSR_JAX:00064) and electroporated mice CD-1 (Charles River) of both sexes at postnatal 4–8 weeks-old. Animals were housed on a 12 hr light-dark cycle with food and water ad libitum.
Viral injection
Request a detailed protocolAfter 1 month postnatal, animals were anesthetized with 2% isoflurane on a head-fixed stereotactic apparatus. After sterilizing the incision site, the skin was opened and, using an FG 1/4 dental drill, a small hole goes thin in the skull over the visual primary cortex (2.5 mm lateral and 0.3 mm anterior from the lambda, 200 μm from pia). We injected 300 nL of the virus at a rate of 30–40 nL/s, using a microsyringe pump (Micro 4), and a Hamilton (7653–01) glass pipette. Once the pipette was placed, we waited 5 min before and after the virus injection and closed the scalp with sutures. Viruses injected were: AAVDJ-CaMKIIa-C1V1(E162T)-TS-p2A-EYFP-WPRE (Stanford University Gene Vector and Virus Core). Mice were used for electrophysiology experiments 2–3 weeks postinjection. These mice were used only to measure evoked EPSC of optogenetically stimulated neurons.
In utero cortical electroporation
Request a detailed protocolPregnant mice (CD-1, Charles River) were placed in a sterile environment following dal Maschio et al., 2012. We performed a laparotomy, under 2% isoflurane anesthesia, and injected (0.5 mg/µL) of pCAG ChroMe-mRuby-ST (Mardinly et al., 2018), ~1 µL per mouse with a glass pipette, in the left lateral ventricle of embryonic day 17 pups. After that, we placed the electrodes on the head 5 mm diameter platinum disk electrodes, (Nepa Gene #CUY650P5) to electroporate with a set of three electrical pulses for pore formation (50 V; duration, 10ms; intervals, 50ms) and then 3 electrical pulses for transferring the plasmid (8 V; duration, 10ms; interval, 50ms). Finally, the embryos were returned to the abdominal cavity and left for their normal development until postnatal day 30.
Brain slices
Request a detailed protocolMice were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine, after transcardial perfusion procedure and posterior cervical dislocation, to obtain brain sagittal slices as described (Ting et al., 2018). Brains were quickly dissected and cooled in a continuously gassed (95% O2 and 5% CO2) icy 194 Sucrose, 30 NaCl, 2.6 KCl, 26 NaHCO3, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 10 glucose, 2 MgSO4, and 0.2 CaCl2 Titrate pH to 7.3–7.4 with HCl. 300 μm thick sagittal slices were cut on a Leica VT1200 S vibratome (Leica Biosystems) and allowed to recover for 1 hr at 34 °C aCSF solution (in mM): 195 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 12 glucose, 1 thiourea, 1 Na-ascorbate, 1 Na-pyruvate, 2 CaCl2, and 1 MgSO4. Titrate pH to 7.3–7.4. Finally, slices were transferred to room temperature.
Patch-clamp recordings
Request a detailed protocolBrain slices were carefully placed in the recording chamber on an upright microscope (Olympus, BX50WI), and continuously perfused with gassed (95% O2 and 5% CO2) aCSF at 5 ml/min. Electrophysiological recordings were performed at 30 degrees, using patch pipettes of borosilicate glass (World Precision Instruments). Pipettes were pulled with a micropipette puller (DMZ-Universal puller) to a 5–6 MOhm as final resistances.
For whole-cell and perforated patch-clamp, patch pipettes were filled with intracellular solution containing the following (in mM): 130 K-gluconate, 20 KCl, 10 HEPES, 10 Na2-phosphocreatine, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.03 Na2-GTP, and titrated to pH 7.3 with KOH. Gramicidin (sigma Aldrich) was dissolved in dimethyl sulfoxide (1 mg /10 µl DMSO) and then dissolved with intracellular solution ~20 ng/ml. To allow membrane sealing, the tip of the pipette was first filled with clean solution and then the pipette was back-filled with a gramicidin-containing solution.
In each case, the patch pipettes were placed in contact with the cell with a MPC-200 micromanipulator (Sutter Instrument). Stimulation and data acquisition were sampled at 10 kHz and low pass filtered at 4 kHz using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices) and Im-Patch open-access software http://impatch.ifc.unam.mx. Recordings were discontinued when leaking current was >25 pA or seal resistance <1 GΩ. Recordings were analyzed with custom routines in MATLAB.
Imaging
Request a detailed protocolTo identify target brain region, we used an upright microscope with a 4 X/0.10 NA air objective (Olympus) before switching to 60 X/0.90 W NA water immersion objective to confirm ChroME or C1V1 opsin expression on target cells. For ChroME, the mRuby3 fluorescent reporter was excited with monochromatic light transmitted through a fiber optic into the microscope (Olympus 100 w high-pressure mercury burner model BH2RFLT3). Emitted fluorescence was band-passed with an Olympus U-49006 ET CY5 filter set: 620/60 excitation filter, 520 dichroic mirrors, and 700/75 emission filter. For C1V1, the EYFP fluorescent reporter was visualized via a 500/24 ex, 520 dichroic mirrors, and a 542/27 em (Semrock). Fluorescence images were acquired (50ms exposure; 10 Hz) using a camera (Orca-ER C4742-95, Hamamatsu) and shutter (UNIBLITZ model VCM-D1) controlled by HCImage software (Hamamatsu). We visualized individual neurons’ fluorescence deep (100 μm) into the brain slice in layer 2/3, evidence of C1V1 or mRuby3 ChroMe expression.
Optogenetic stimulation
Request a detailed protocolOptogenetic stimulation of ChroMe opsin was performed with a 470 nm fiber-coupled LED (M470F1, Thorlabs), fiber optic cannula (M79L01, Thorlabs), and LED driver (M00329012, Thorlabs). Optogenetic stimulation of C1V1 was performed using a 617 nm fiber-coupled LED (M617F2, Thorlabs), fiber optic cannula (CFM14L10, Thorlabs), and LED driver (DC2200, Thorlabs). The optogenetic stimulation protocol consisted of trains of 10 Hz, 5ms light pulses for 4 s followed by a 10 s rest, we repeated the same stimulus during 15–80 min to mimic stimulation conditions previously used in vivo experiments (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016) to co-stimulate neurons.
Only neurons that exhibited action potentials in response to each LED pulse were included in the data analysis of optogenetically stimulated neurons. LED intensity applied throughout the protocol was the lowest necessary for inducing action potentials. For optogenetically stimulated neurons, parameters were monitored before and 15–80 min after the stimulation protocol. Optogenetic stimulation protocol was stopped every 15 or 20 min to monitor parameters, and then restarted. For unstimulated neurons, the same parameters were monitored at the beginning and then every 15–20 min for 80 min approximately.
We observed that neurons expressing opsin often exhibited small alterations in intrinsic properties. Specifically, the firing rate was higher in neurons with opsins than in control cells, and the membrane resistance and membrane potential become higher and more variable than neurons without opsin expression.
Experimental design
Request a detailed protocolTo co-activate neurons (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016), we delivered C1V1 or ChroME opsin in pyramidal neurons (Mardinly et al., 2018). No significant differences were found in experiments with both opsins and data were pooled together. We localized layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons in primary visual cortex of brain slices expressing opsin by virus injection and in utero electroporation (Figure 1A). To test whether the co-stimulation of connected neurons reinforced or created new connections (Carrillo-Reid et al., 2016), we examined pairs of connected and unconnected neurons (Figure 1B–C). We recorded and characterized evoked excitatory postsynaptic currents and potentials (EPSCs and EPSPs). We also recorded passive and active electrical properties of neurons using perforated patch-clamp recording. After measuring these parameters in control condition, we began the optogenetic or electrical stimulation protocol (Figure 1D and E). For optogenetic stimulation, action potentials were generated by trains of 10 Hz, 5ms light pulses for 4 s followed by a 10 s rest (Figure 1D). For electrical stimulation protocol, action potentials were generated with 10 Hz trains, 5–10ms depolarizing currents pulses 400–600 pA for 4 s followed by a 10 s rest (Figure 1E). During these experiments, we noticed that the time after optogenetic or electrical stimulation was important, immediately after stimulation, the membrane activity of stimulated neurons became unstable, particularly during the first 3 min after stimulation. After that, membrane potential recovered. Thus, we evaluated membrane parameters 3–5 min post-stimulation and synaptic parameters 10 min post-stimulation.
Statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolStatistical details are showing in each figure legends. Group data are expressed as median ± sem. Wilcoxon and Mann-Whitney test were used for nonparametric analysis. Differences between two groups were considered significant when *p<0.05, **p<0.01 and ***p<0.001. All statistics were performed using statistical functions in MATLAB.
Data availability
Data have been deposited with Dryad (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.j6q573ngc).
-
Dryad Digital RepositoryIntrinsic excitability mechanisms of neuronal ensemble formation.https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.j6q573ngc
References
-
Long-term potentiation of intrinsic excitability at the mossy fiber-granule cell synapse of rat cerebellumThe Journal of Neuroscience 20:5208–5216.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-14-05208.2000
-
Ionic mechanisms underlying repetitive high-frequency burst firing in supragranular cortical neuronsThe Journal of Neuroscience 20:4829–4843.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-13-04829.2000
-
CORTICAL PLASTICITY: from synapses to mapsAnnual Review of Neuroscience 21:149–186.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.21.1.149
-
Downregulation of dendritic I(h) in CA1 pyramidal neurons after LTPThe Journal of Neuroscience 28:8635–8643.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1411-08.2008
-
Imprinting and recalling cortical ensemblesScience (New York, N.Y.) 353:691–694.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf7560
-
Terfenadine, a Nonsedating AntihistamineDrug Intelligence & Clinical Pharmacy 19:812–817.https://doi.org/10.1177/106002808501901103
-
BookThe Computational Brain., The Computational Brain., Computational NeuroscienceCambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press.https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/2010.001.0001
-
Variance and invariance of neuronal long-term representationsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 372:20160161.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0161
-
Long-term potentiation of intrinsic excitability in LV visual cortical neuronsJournal of Neurophysiology 92:341–348.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01059.2003
-
Plasticity of intrinsic neuronal excitabilityCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 54:73–82.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2018.09.001
-
Plasticity in the intrinsic excitability of cortical pyramidal neuronsNature Neuroscience 2:515–520.https://doi.org/10.1038/9165
-
Learning, aging and intrinsic neuronal plasticityTrends in Neurosciences 29:587–599.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2006.08.005
-
Time to rethink the neural mechanisms of learning and memoryNeurobiology of Learning and Memory 108:136–144.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.11.019
-
Why is synaptic plasticity not enough?Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 176:107336.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107336
-
Pyramidal cell communication within local networks in layer 2/3 of rat neocortexThe Journal of Physiology 551:139–153.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2003.044784
-
Synfire chains and cortical songs: temporal modules of cortical activityScience (New York, N.Y.) 304:559–564.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1093173
-
Spatial profile of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic connectivity in mouse primary auditory cortexThe Journal of Neuroscience 32:5609–5619.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5158-11.2012
-
Bursts as a unit of neural information: making unreliable synapses reliableTrends in Neurosciences 20:38–43.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(96)10070-9
-
Studies on the structure of the cerebral cortex. I. The area entorhinalisJournal Für Psychologie Und Neurologie 46:113–177.
-
Analysis of the activity of the chains of internuncial neuronsJournal of Neurophysiology 1:207–244.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1938.1.3.207
-
Variability, compensation and homeostasis in neuron and network functionNature Reviews. Neuroscience 7:563–574.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1949
-
Precise multimodal optical control of neural ensemble activityNature Neuroscience 21:881–893.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0139-8
-
Physiology and anatomy of synaptic connections between thick tufted pyramidal neurones in the developing rat neocortexThe Journal of Physiology 500 (Pt 2):409–440.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1997.sp022031
-
Cortical layer-specific critical dynamics triggering perceptionScience (New York, N.Y.) 365:eaaw5202.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaw5202
-
Mediation of hippocampal mossy fiber long-term potentiation by presynaptic Ih channelsScience (New York, N.Y.) 295:143–147.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1064285
-
Highly differentiated projection-specific cortical subnetworksThe Journal of Neuroscience 31:10380–10391.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0772-11.2011
-
D.O. Hebb: The Organization of Behavior, Wiley: New York; 1949Brain Research Bulletin 50:437.https://doi.org/10.1016/s0361-9230(99)00182-3
-
The columnar organization of the neocortexBrain 120 (Pt 4):701–722.https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.4.701
-
The h channel mediates location dependence and plasticity of intrinsic phase response in rat hippocampal neuronsThe Journal of Neuroscience 28:5846–5860.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0835-08.2008
-
Multiple forms of activity-dependent intrinsic plasticity in layer V cortical neurones in vivoThe Journal of Physiology 587:3189–3205.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2009.169334
-
Memory: Engram cells retain memory under retrograde amnesiaScience (New York, N.Y.) 348:1007–1013.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaa5542
-
Metastability of active CA3 networksThe Journal of Neuroscience 27:517–528.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4514-06.2007
-
Vesicle pools and short-term synaptic depression: lessons from a large synapseTrends in Neurosciences 25:206–212.https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-2236(02)02139-2
-
BookHandbook of Brain MicrocircuitsOxford University Press.https://doi.org/10.1093/med/9780195389883.001.0001
-
Dendritic excitability and synaptic plasticityPhysiological Reviews 88:769–840.https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00016.2007
-
Preparation of Acute Brain Slices Using an OptimizedJournal of Visualized Experiments 1:e825.https://doi.org/10.3791/53825
-
Memory engram storage and retrievalCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 35:101–109.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.07.009
-
Activity-dependent changes in the intrinsic properties of cultured neuronsScience (New York, N.Y.) 264:974–977.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.8178157
-
Morphological and physiological characterization of pyramidal neuron subtypes in rat medial prefrontal cortexCerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y 25:788–805.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht278
-
Plasticity and stability of visual field maps in adult primary visual cortexNature Reviews. Neuroscience 10:873–884.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2741
-
EFFECTS OF VISUAL DEPRIVATION ON MORPHOLOGY AND PHYSIOLOGY OF CELLS IN THE CATS LATERAL GENICULATE BODYJournal of Neurophysiology 26:978–993.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1963.26.6.978
-
Activity-dependent long-term potentiation of intrinsic excitability in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neuronsThe Journal of Neuroscience 25:1750–1760.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4217-04.2005
-
Purkinje cell intrinsic excitability increases after synaptic long term depressionJournal of Neurophysiology 116:1208–1217.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00369.2016
-
CREB, cellular excitability, and cognition: Implications for agingBehavioural Brain Research 322:206–211.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.07.042
-
From the neuron doctrine to neural networksNature Reviews. Neuroscience 16:487–497.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3962
-
Electrical activity and development of neural circuitsNature Neuroscience 4 Suppl:1207–1214.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn753
-
The other side of the engram: experience-driven changes in neuronal intrinsic excitabilityNature Reviews. Neuroscience 4:885–900.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1248
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institute of Mental Health (R01EY011787)
- Rafael Yuste
National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH115900)
- Rafael Yuste
Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología (287725)
- Tzitzitlini Alejandre-García
Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología (CVU365863)
- Jesús Pérez-Ortega
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank James Holland, for his assistance and members of the Yuste Lab for useful comments. Supported by R01EY011787 and R01MH115900. TA and JP have postdoctoral fellowships from the National Council of Science and Technology from Mexico (CONACYT). SK and RY conceived the project. SK and TA performed experiments and TA and RY wrote the paper. TA, SK and JP analyzed the data. All authors planned experiments, discussed results and edited the paper. RY assembled and directed the team and secured funding and resources.
Ethics
All procedures were performed by following the U.S. National Institutes of Health and Columbia University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines (IACUC, Protocol #AC-AAAV3464).
Copyright
© 2022, Alejandre-García et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 4,881
- views
-
- 968
- downloads
-
- 27
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Cell Biology
- Neuroscience
Chronic high-fat feeding triggers metabolic dysfunction including obesity, insulin resistance, and diabetes. How high-fat intake first triggers these pathophysiological states remains unknown. Here, we identify an acute microglial metabolic response that rapidly translates intake of high-fat diet (HFD) to a surprisingly beneficial effect on metabolism and spatial/learning memory. High-fat intake rapidly increases palmitate levels in cerebrospinal fluid and triggers a wave of microglial metabolic activation characterized by mitochondrial membrane activation and fission as well as metabolic skewing toward aerobic glycolysis. These effects are detectable throughout the brain and can be detected within as little as 12 hr of HFD exposure. In vivo, microglial ablation and conditional DRP1 deletion show that the microglial metabolic response is necessary for the acute effects of HFD. 13C-tracing experiments reveal that in addition to processing via β-oxidation, microglia shunt a substantial fraction of palmitate toward anaplerosis and re-release of bioenergetic carbons into the extracellular milieu in the form of lactate, glutamate, succinate, and intriguingly, the neuroprotective metabolite itaconate. Together, these data identify microglia as a critical nutrient regulatory node in the brain, metabolizing away harmful fatty acids and liberating the same carbons as alternate bioenergetic and protective substrates for surrounding cells. The data identify a surprisingly beneficial effect of short-term HFD on learning and memory.
-
- Cell Biology
- Chromosomes and Gene Expression
During oncogene-induced senescence there are striking changes in the organisation of heterochromatin in the nucleus. This is accompanied by activation of a pro-inflammatory gene expression programme - the senescence associated secretory phenotype (SASP) - driven by transcription factors such as NF-κB. The relationship between heterochromatin re-organisation and the SASP has been unclear. Here we show that TPR, a protein of the nuclear pore complex basket required for heterochromatin re-organisation during senescence, is also required for the very early activation of NF-κB signalling during the stress-response phase of oncogene-induced senescence. This is prior to activation of the SASP and occurs without affecting NF-κB nuclear import. We show that TPR is required for the activation of innate immune signalling at these early stages of senescence and we link this to the formation of heterochromatin-enriched cytoplasmic chromatin fragments thought to bleb off from the nuclear periphery. We show that HMGA1 is also required for cytoplasmic chromatin fragment formation. Together these data suggest that re-organisation of heterochromatin is involved in altered structural integrity of the nuclear periphery during senescence, and that this can lead to activation of cytoplasmic nucleic acid sensing, NF-κB signalling, and activation of the SASP.