Evolutionary shaping of human brain dynamics

  1. James C Pang  Is a corresponding author
  2. James K Rilling
  3. James A Roberts
  4. Martijn van den Heuvel,
  5. Luca Cocchi  Is a corresponding author
  1. Monash University, Australia
  2. Emory University, United States
  3. QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Australia
  4. Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Netherlands

Abstract

The human brain is distinct from those of other species in terms of size, organization, and connectivity. How do structural evolutionary differences drive patterns of neural activity enabling brain function? Here, we combine brain imaging and biophysical modeling to show that the anatomical wiring of the human brain distinctly shapes neural dynamics. This shaping is characterized by a narrower distribution of dynamic ranges across brain regions compared with that of chimpanzees, our closest living primate relatives. We find that such a narrow dynamic range distribution supports faster integration between regions, particularly in transmodal systems. Conversely, a broad dynamic range distribution as seen in chimpanzees facilitates brain processes relying more on neural interactions within specialized local brain systems. These findings suggest that human brain dynamics have evolved to foster rapid associative processes in service of complex cognitive functions and behavior.

Data availability

All source data and MATLAB codes to perform sample simulations, analyze results, and generate the main and supplementary figures of this study are openly available at https://github.com/jchrispang/evolution-brain-tuning.

The following previously published data sets were used

Article and author information

Author details

  1. James C Pang

    Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, Australia
    For correspondence
    James.Pang1@monash.edu
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-2461-2760
  2. James K Rilling

    Department of Anthropology, Emory University, Atlanta, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. James A Roberts

    QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Queensland, Australia
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Martijn van den Heuvel,

    Department of Complex Traits Genetics, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Luca Cocchi

    Systems Neuroscience Group, QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, Australia
    For correspondence
    Luca.Cocchi@qimrberghofer.edu.au
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-3651-2676

Funding

National Health and Medical Research Council (11144936)

  • James A Roberts

National Health and Medical Research Council (1145168)

  • James A Roberts

Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (ALWOP.179)

  • Martijn van den Heuvel,

Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (VIDI (452-16-015))

  • Martijn van den Heuvel,

European Research Council (Consolidator grant 101001062)

  • Martijn van den Heuvel,

National Health and Medical Research Council (1138711)

  • Luca Cocchi

National Health and Medical Research Council (2001283)

  • Luca Cocchi

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All data were taken from previously published studies and were approved by the respective oversighting ethics committees. Procedures were carried out in accordance with protocols approved by the Yerkes National Primate Research Center and the Emory University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (YER-2001206).

Human subjects: All data were taken from previously published studies and were approved by the respective oversighting ethics committees. Procedures were carried out in accordance with protocols approved by the Yerkes National Primate Research Center and the Emory University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (YER-2001206). All humans were recruited as healthy volunteers with no known neurological conditions and provided informed consent (IRB00000028).

Copyright

© 2022, Pang et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 2,181
    views
  • 395
    downloads
  • 7
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. James C Pang
  2. James K Rilling
  3. James A Roberts
  4. Martijn van den Heuvel,
  5. Luca Cocchi
(2022)
Evolutionary shaping of human brain dynamics
eLife 11:e80627.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.80627

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.80627

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Sven Ohl, Martin Rolfs
    Research Article

    Detecting causal relations structures our perception of events in the world. Here, we determined for visual interactions whether generalized (i.e. feature-invariant) or specialized (i.e. feature-selective) visual routines underlie the perception of causality. To this end, we applied a visual adaptation protocol to assess the adaptability of specific features in classical launching events of simple geometric shapes. We asked observers to report whether they observed a launch or a pass in ambiguous test events (i.e. the overlap between two discs varied from trial to trial). After prolonged exposure to causal launch events (the adaptor) defined by a particular set of features (i.e. a particular motion direction, motion speed, or feature conjunction), observers were less likely to see causal launches in subsequent ambiguous test events than before adaptation. Crucially, adaptation was contingent on the causal impression in launches as demonstrated by a lack of adaptation in non-causal control events. We assessed whether this negative aftereffect transfers to test events with a new set of feature values that were not presented during adaptation. Processing in specialized (as opposed to generalized) visual routines predicts that the transfer of visual adaptation depends on the feature similarity of the adaptor and the test event. We show that the negative aftereffects do not transfer to unadapted launch directions but do transfer to launch events of different speeds. Finally, we used colored discs to assign distinct feature-based identities to the launching and the launched stimulus. We found that the adaptation transferred across colors if the test event had the same motion direction as the adaptor. In summary, visual adaptation allowed us to carve out a visual feature space underlying the perception of causality and revealed specialized visual routines that are tuned to a launch’s motion direction.

    1. Neuroscience
    Ulrike Pech, Jasper Janssens ... Patrik Verstreken
    Research Article

    The classical diagnosis of Parkinsonism is based on motor symptoms that are the consequence of nigrostriatal pathway dysfunction and reduced dopaminergic output. However, a decade prior to the emergence of motor issues, patients frequently experience non-motor symptoms, such as a reduced sense of smell (hyposmia). The cellular and molecular bases for these early defects remain enigmatic. To explore this, we developed a new collection of five fruit fly models of familial Parkinsonism and conducted single-cell RNA sequencing on young brains of these models. Interestingly, cholinergic projection neurons are the most vulnerable cells, and genes associated with presynaptic function are the most deregulated. Additional single nucleus sequencing of three specific brain regions of Parkinson’s disease patients confirms these findings. Indeed, the disturbances lead to early synaptic dysfunction, notably affecting cholinergic olfactory projection neurons crucial for olfactory function in flies. Correcting these defects specifically in olfactory cholinergic interneurons in flies or inducing cholinergic signaling in Parkinson mutant human induced dopaminergic neurons in vitro using nicotine, both rescue age-dependent dopaminergic neuron decline. Hence, our research uncovers that one of the earliest indicators of disease in five different models of familial Parkinsonism is synaptic dysfunction in higher-order cholinergic projection neurons and this contributes to the development of hyposmia. Furthermore, the shared pathways of synaptic failure in these cholinergic neurons ultimately contribute to dopaminergic dysfunction later in life.