Neuroscience: Unveiling hidden sources of noise
In March 2014, a team of cosmologists made headlines around the world when they reported possible evidence for cosmic inflation – an extremely short period during which the universe rapidly expanded immediately after the Big Bang (Ade et al., 2014). However, it later emerged that some of the signal this result was based on had been produced by something seemingly quite insignificant: cosmic dust (Cowen, 2015). This episode underscored how small, apparently innocuous factors can have colossal implications when studying the universe. The same is true in neuroscience, with perhaps even higher stakes.
We are all impacted by changes in our brains, especially in the latter part of our lives (Alzheimer’s Association, 2019). As the global population ages at an unprecedented rate, neuroscientists are leveraging a vast array of tools to confront the complexities of brain aging at every biological scale. When recording brain activity, these researchers must exhibit the same precision demanded of cosmologists, carefully accounting for the various sources of non-neural noise stemming from their instruments, from their lab environment, or from biological signals such as eye movements or skin conductance. Now, in eLife, Fabian Schmidt and colleagues at Paris Lodron University of Salzburg and other institutions in Austria report that cardiac signals may be contaminating recordings thought to be of neural origin (Schmidt et al., 2024). Just as space dust misled cosmologists, electrical activity from the heart might distort our understanding of brain aging by influencing what is thought to be purely ‘neural’ data.
To understand these results, it is important to know that brain activity is composed of electrical signals that are either periodic (rhythmic) or aperiodic (non-rhythmic). Periodic or ‘neural’ oscillations occur at regular intervals, showing up as consistent patterns or waves. Extensively studied over the past century, these are linked to cognitive processes, perception, and various disease states (Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004). Aperiodic activity, in contrast, does not follow a predictable pattern. Historically, it has largely been treated as noise to be averaged away in analyses, but mounting evidence suggests it may in fact be a signal in its own right. Crucially, previous studies have shown that neural aperiodic activity changes with age (Donoghue et al., 2020). However, cardiac signals contain both periodic and aperiodic components as well.
The work by Schmidt et al., which relied on datasets from various institutions, involved examining electrocardiograms (ECG) as well as neural data from electroencephalography (EEG) and resting state magnetoencephalography (MEG) – 1104 and 1282 recordings, respectively – obtained from individuals aged 18–92. The team extracted the aperiodic signal from both the neural and cardiac datasets and explored how some of its features evolved with age.
Their analyses revealed that cardiac artifacts significantly influenced the age-related changes in aperiodic activity detected in brain recordings. In fact, certain MEG and EEG changes could be attributed to the aging of the heart rather than of the brain, as had previously been thought.
Schmidt et al. then showed that attempting to ‘clean’ this cardiac contamination using a standard method called independent component analysis was insufficient. Despite the approach attempting to separate cardiac contributions from the neural data, residual aperiodic signals from the heart could still be detected in MEG and EEG recordings thought to capture only brain activity.
Moreover, results across different datasets pointed to a complex pattern of age-related changes in aperiodic neural activity. Researchers often examine how much of the electrical activity (or ‘power’) from the brain or heart is concentrated across various frequencies – with the slope of the resulting curve, which is how aperiodic activity is often measured, usually changing with age. In the brain, this curve is steeper in youth (as lower frequencies concentrate more power) and flattens in old age, possibly due to an increase in neural noise (Voytek et al., 2015; Figure 1). This effect could potentially be driven by the heart, as Schmidt et al. found a similar age-dependent flattening pattern in the cardiac aperiodic activity leaking into brain recordings. However, an opposite pattern emerged in one of the ECG datasets, with cardiac aperiodic activity steepening rather than flattening with age in a specific lower frequency range (0.25–12 Hz). This observation was particularly surprising given the flattening trend seen in other frequency ranges and datasets; it highlights how different physiological processes may drive age-related changes in aperiodic activity in a frequency-dependent and nuanced manner.
Age and disease are not alone in affecting aperiodic activity, as cognitive and behavioral factors can also have an influence. For instance, recordings show that neural aperiodic activity flattens when subjects perform cognitive tasks (Preston et al., 2022). Schmidt et al. detected a similar effect in the cardiac aperiodic activity of participants temporarily holding information in mind during a working memory task, suggesting that cardiac influence on neural changes may extend beyond aging.
Where do we go from here? The findings by Schmidt et al. underscore the importance of developing more sophisticated models that can accurately account for all potential sources of noise. By doing so, we can avoid being misled by our own dust; instead, we can move beyond basic observations about ‘something happening in the brain’ and towards a deeper physiological understanding of how neural circuits are changing and interacting with other age-related bodily processes.
References
-
Detection of B-mode polarization at degree angular scales by BICEP2Physical Review Letters 112:241101.https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevLett.112.241101
-
2019 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figuresAlzheimer’s & Dementia 15:321–387.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2019.01.010
-
Neuronal oscillations in cortical networksScience 304:1926–1929.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1099745
-
Parameterizing neural power spectra into periodic and aperiodic componentsNature Neuroscience 23:1655–1665.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-00744-x
-
Age-related changes in 1/f neural electrophysiological noiseThe Journal of Neuroscience 35:13257–13265.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2332-14.2015
Article and author information
Author details
Publication history
Copyright
© 2024, Fitzgerald et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,150
- views
-
- 75
- downloads
-
- 0
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Medicine
- Neuroscience
The advent of midazolam holds profound implications for modern clinical practice. The hypnotic and sedative effects of midazolam afford it broad clinical applicability. However, the specific mechanisms underlying the modulation of altered consciousness by midazolam remain elusive. Herein, using pharmacology, optogenetics, chemogenetics, fiber photometry, and gene knockdown, this in vivo research revealed the role of locus coeruleus (LC)-ventrolateral preoptic nucleus noradrenergic neural circuit in regulating midazolam-induced altered consciousness. This effect was mediated by α1 adrenergic receptors. Moreover, gamma-aminobutyric acid receptor type A (GABAA-R) represents a mechanistically crucial binding site in the LC for midazolam. These findings will provide novel insights into the neural circuit mechanisms underlying the recovery of consciousness after midazolam administration and will help guide the timing of clinical dosing and propose effective intervention targets for timely recovery from midazolam-induced loss of consciousness.
-
- Neuroscience
Motivation depends on dopamine, but might be modulated by acetylcholine which influences dopamine release in the striatum, and amplifies motivation in animal studies. A corresponding effect in humans would be important clinically, since anticholinergic drugs are frequently used in Parkinson’s disease, a condition that can also disrupt motivation. Reward and dopamine make us more ready to respond, as indexed by reaction times (RT), and move faster, sometimes termed vigour. These effects may be controlled by preparatory processes that can be tracked using electroencephalography (EEG). We measured vigour in a placebo-controlled, double-blinded study of trihexyphenidyl (THP), a muscarinic antagonist, with an incentivised eye movement task and EEG. Participants responded faster and with greater vigour when incentives were high, but THP blunted these motivational effects, suggesting that muscarinic receptors facilitate invigoration by reward. Preparatory EEG build-up (contingent negative variation [CNV]) was strengthened by high incentives and by muscarinic blockade, although THP reduced the incentive effect. The amplitude of preparatory activity predicted both vigour and RT, although over distinct scalp regions; frontal activity predicted vigour, whereas a larger, earlier, central component predicted RT. The incentivisation of RT was partly mediated by the CNV, though vigour was not. Moreover, the CNV mediated the drug’s effect on dampening incentives, suggesting that muscarinic receptors underlie the motivational influence on this preparatory activity. Taken together, these findings show that a muscarinic blocker impairs motivated action in healthy people, and that medial frontal preparatory neural activity mediates this for RT.