Memory: Integrating past experiences

New results help address a longstanding debate regarding which learning strategies allow animals to anticipate negative events based on past associations between sensory stimuli.
  1. Thomas MW Leir
  2. Matthew PH Gardner  Is a corresponding author
  1. Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts and Sciences, Concordia University, Canada

The brain’s ability to integrate distinct memories is critical for forming accurate predictions in unfamiliar situations. This process can be studied in the laboratory using a three-stage preconditioning task (Brogden, 1939). First, animals learn to associate two ‘neutral’ stimuli through a sequential presentation, such as a sound followed by a light. Next, the latter stimulus (for instance, the light) is followed by a motivationally meaningful experience, such as a fear-inducing foot shock. The final stage tests how the animals respond to the neutral stimulus not directly associated with the negative experience (in this case, the sound). Typically, the animals behave as if this signal also predicts the foot shock, showing that they have integrated information from the neutral association (between sound and light) with that of the motivational association (between light and shock).

Two conflicting perspectives exist on how this integration occurs (Gostolupce et al., 2022; Holmes et al., 2022; Rizley and Rescorla, 1972). The mediated theory proposes that integration occurs at the second stage: the neutral association formed in the first stage allows the animals to ‘picture’ the sound signal as the light is shown during the second stage, permitting development of an association between the sound and the shock (Iordanova et al., 2011; Wong et al., 2019). Alternatively, integration could occur during the test phase through a process known as chaining; the animals ‘think’ of the light when they hear the sound signal and therefore recall the shock, chaining together these separate sound-light and light-shock associations. Now, in eLife, Nathan Holmes and colleagues from the University of New South Wales – including Francesca Wong as first author – report that both chaining and mediated learning are plausible mechanisms for integrating this information, but that each mechanism is used in different situations (Wong et al., 2025).

The team focused on two brain regions: the perirhinal cortex, which forms the neutral stimulus associations, and the basolateral amygdala, which supports the fear associations (Holmes et al., 2013). In 2019, three members of the team showed that neural plasticity in the perirhinal cortex depends on a receptor known as NDMA and is required during the second stage of preconditioning. This indicated that integration could potentially take place through mediated learning during this stage (Wong et al., 2019).

Building on this previous work, Wong et al. used the sensory preconditioning task to train rats either extensively or briefly during stage one, which meant that the two groups differed in the number of sound-light pairings they experienced. NMDA receptors in the perirhinal cortex were then pharmacologically blocked during stage two when the light was paired with the foot shock, which should interfere with mediated conditioning, but not the chaining method. Supporting their previous findings, this resulted in a decreased response to the sound stimulus during the test phase. However, this only occurred in rats for which stage one had been brief. This suggests that rats with limited sound-light training used mediated conditioning, while those with extensive training likely engaged in the chaining method at test (Figure 1).

Proposed mechanisms of learning during brief or extended preconditioning training.

Wong et al. used a preconditioning task to study how rats learn. In stage 1 (top), rats were exposed to a neutral stimulus pairing of sound followed by a light. In stage 2 (middle), the light was followed with an electric shock, which represents a negative outcome. In stage 3 (bottom), rats were presented with the neutral sound signal from stage 1. The rats associated this neutral stimulus with the electric shock and acted fearfully, even though the sound stimulus had not previously been presented with the electric shock. The stimuli were counterbalanced across all experiments, so that half of the rats in each group received the training in the order shown in this figure and half were trained with light-sound pairings in stage 1, sound-shock pairings in stage 2 and a light test in stage 3. Varying how many times the rats are presented with the neutral stimulus pairings in Stage 1 dictates the method they use to integrate associations between the neutral stimuli and the subsequent unpleasant electric shock. After brief training (left) rats use a mediated learning mechanism during stage 2 to associate the sound with the electric shock. With extended training (right), rather than carrying out mediated learning during stage 2, associations are made during the test phase (stage 3), using the chaining technique. This supports the theory that the sound elicits a prediction of the light and subsequent shock through reactivation of the memories stored during stage 1 and stage 2, respectively.

Next, to investigate whether communication between the perirhinal cortex and the basal amygdala was required for this integration, Wong et al. inactivated each region on opposite sides of the brain. This standard procedure for determining functional connectivity between brain regions relies on the observation that connections are primarily within a single hemisphere. If communication between the brain regions is required, inactivating the regions in different hemispheres prevents information from being processed in either hemisphere. The experiments revealed that communication was always required for integration. When this process took place, however, varied in line with the findings from the previous experiments. If stage one training was short, then communication between the two regions was required during stage two, when mediated learning theoretically occurs. On the other hand, if stage one was extended, this communication was required during the test stage, when chaining theoretically occurs.

Taken together, these findings help to reconcile the debate between mediated integration and chaining. However, it remains unclear why the training duration affects the mechanism of learning. Wong et al. suggest that the additional training in stage one strengthens the temporal relationship between the neutral stimuli (sound followed by light). This prevents mediated learning, which requires a reverse temporal sequence (where exposure to light triggers the thought of sound) to facilitate the sound-shock association (Figure 1). On the other hand, this additional training makes chaining possible, since temporal associations can strongly predict the order of events. Future research is required to determine whether this argument can explain the effect or whether other mechanisms may contribute to this type of learning.

References

    1. Brogden WJ
    (1939) Sensory pre-conditioning
    Journal of Experimental Psychology 25:323–332.
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0058944

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Thomas MW Leir

    Thomas MW Leir is in the Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts and Sciences, Concordia University, Montreal, Canada

    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0009-0000-0812-5834
  2. Matthew PH Gardner

    Matthew PH Gardner is in the Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts and Sciences, Concordia University, Montreal, Canada

    For correspondence
    matthew.gardner@concordia.ca
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-9146-5043

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published:

Copyright

© 2025, Leir and Gardner

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 208
    views
  • 19
    downloads
  • 0
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Thomas MW Leir
  2. Matthew PH Gardner
(2025)
Memory: Integrating past experiences
eLife 14:e106291.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.106291
  1. Further reading

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Sven Ohl, Martin Rolfs
    Research Article

    Detecting causal relations structures our perception of events in the world. Here, we determined for visual interactions whether generalized (i.e. feature-invariant) or specialized (i.e. feature-selective) visual routines underlie the perception of causality. To this end, we applied a visual adaptation protocol to assess the adaptability of specific features in classical launching events of simple geometric shapes. We asked observers to report whether they observed a launch or a pass in ambiguous test events (i.e. the overlap between two discs varied from trial to trial). After prolonged exposure to causal launch events (the adaptor) defined by a particular set of features (i.e. a particular motion direction, motion speed, or feature conjunction), observers were less likely to see causal launches in subsequent ambiguous test events than before adaptation. Crucially, adaptation was contingent on the causal impression in launches as demonstrated by a lack of adaptation in non-causal control events. We assessed whether this negative aftereffect transfers to test events with a new set of feature values that were not presented during adaptation. Processing in specialized (as opposed to generalized) visual routines predicts that the transfer of visual adaptation depends on the feature similarity of the adaptor and the test event. We show that the negative aftereffects do not transfer to unadapted launch directions but do transfer to launch events of different speeds. Finally, we used colored discs to assign distinct feature-based identities to the launching and the launched stimulus. We found that the adaptation transferred across colors if the test event had the same motion direction as the adaptor. In summary, visual adaptation allowed us to carve out a visual feature space underlying the perception of causality and revealed specialized visual routines that are tuned to a launch’s motion direction.

    1. Neuroscience
    Ulrike Pech, Jasper Janssens ... Patrik Verstreken
    Research Article

    The classical diagnosis of Parkinsonism is based on motor symptoms that are the consequence of nigrostriatal pathway dysfunction and reduced dopaminergic output. However, a decade prior to the emergence of motor issues, patients frequently experience non-motor symptoms, such as a reduced sense of smell (hyposmia). The cellular and molecular bases for these early defects remain enigmatic. To explore this, we developed a new collection of five fruit fly models of familial Parkinsonism and conducted single-cell RNA sequencing on young brains of these models. Interestingly, cholinergic projection neurons are the most vulnerable cells, and genes associated with presynaptic function are the most deregulated. Additional single nucleus sequencing of three specific brain regions of Parkinson’s disease patients confirms these findings. Indeed, the disturbances lead to early synaptic dysfunction, notably affecting cholinergic olfactory projection neurons crucial for olfactory function in flies. Correcting these defects specifically in olfactory cholinergic interneurons in flies or inducing cholinergic signaling in Parkinson mutant human induced dopaminergic neurons in vitro using nicotine, both rescue age-dependent dopaminergic neuron decline. Hence, our research uncovers that one of the earliest indicators of disease in five different models of familial Parkinsonism is synaptic dysfunction in higher-order cholinergic projection neurons and this contributes to the development of hyposmia. Furthermore, the shared pathways of synaptic failure in these cholinergic neurons ultimately contribute to dopaminergic dysfunction later in life.